Mskongsclassroom.weebly.com



MemoryLesson 4- Long-term memory: RetrievalGo to Activity 4.1- Constructive Memory/Schemas: The Rumor ChainRate of forgettingRetrieval is the process of getting (“retrieving”) information out of memory storage.Forgetting does not occur at a constant rate.Ebbinghaus conducted experiments with nonsense syllabi. In his studies, he demonstrated that most forgetting occurs early. The rate of forgetting slows over time.Bahrick (1984) has confirmed this pattern using more naturalistic materials. Bahrick examined memory for math and foreign language learned in high school and for the names and faces of high school classmates. He assessed forgetting across several decades and demonstrated that most forgetting occurs early, and that some information persists over time. The research shows what you remember after 3 years, you are likely to remember for the rest of your life.Proactive and retroactive interferenceInterference, which occurs when some information blocks or disrupts the recall of other information, is believed to be a primary source of forgetting. There are two types of interference: retroactive and proactive.Retroactive interference occurs when new information blocks or disrupts retrieval of older information. For example, if you are trying to recall an old, obsolete password to an account but can only remember your current password, you are experiencing retroactive interference.Proactive interference occurs when old, previously learned memories intrude with the recall of newer memories. For example, imagine your favorite pizza place moves across town and changes phone numbers. If you drive to the old location or call the old phone number, you are experiencing proactive interference.Proactive interference highlights the importance of forgetting in our everyday lives. Failing to forget information that has become obsolete will disrupt and impair memory for current information.For example, thinking about where you parked your car yesterday may lead you to the wrong space today. In a similar vein, it’s important to call your current boyfriend by his actual name and not by your previous boyfriend’s name.Note. These two concepts are frequently confused by students. Teachers may want to reference the idea that “the problem is with the prefix” or use a mnemonic to help students remember the difference between these two terms.The fan effect is another example in which remembering too much can cause forgetting.When we associate a number of memories with one cue, we are slower and less accurate in retrieving any one of those memories than we are if we associate only one memory with a cue.For example, if we have to remember John’s office phone number, his cell phone number, his home phone number, and his wife’s phone number, we are less effective in recalling any one of those numbers than if we simply have to remember his cell number.To reduce interference, students should try associating fewer items with any one cue.Retrieval-induced forgetting occurs when we have many memories associated with a cue, and we selectively rehearse only one or two of those memories.For example, if we know John’s work, cell, and home numbers but repeatedly call only his cell, our memory for his other numbers will diminish.Practicing all the items associated with a single cue will help reduce this interference. For example, if we are going to dial John’s cell, we might also rehearse his other phone numbers before we dial his cell.Encoding specificity and retrieval cuesEncoding specificity, a concept introduced by Tulving, states that the most effective cues at retrieval are those that match the cues present at encoding.Tulving (1983) makes the case that much of our forgetting is due to problems in accessibility or the failure to retrieve memories that still reside in long-term memory. (This contrasts with forgetting due to problems in availability, where the memory is simply no longer stored in the brain.)A particularly frustrating example of a memory failure due to an accessibility problem occurs when we are in a tip-of-thetongue state. In that case, we are aware we know the information that we are searching for, but we simply cannot access it.More generally, there are many situations in which memories are available in long-term memory; but we have difficulty accessing them (e.g., the name of a well-known actor, some of the details of a vacation we took last year) until we are given a good cue (e.g., the first name of the actor, a photograph of us playing miniature golf while on vacation), which, according to the encoding specificity principle, is a cue that gets us to think about the event like we did when we encoded it.Examples:Consider the prospective memory task of walking into the kitchen from the bedroom and then forgetting why you were going to the kitchen. A good way to remember your intention is to get back to what you were doing when you formed the intention—that is, to walk back into the bedroom.Other good examples for high school students include how the smell of suntan lotion can bring back memories of a family beach trip or how a song can instantly bring back memories of summer camp or time spent with friends.Context effectsChanging the physical context of the studying task from the physical context of the testing task can impair memory.In a study by Godden & Baddeley (1975), for example, individuals learned best when they studied and were tested on land or studied and were tested while scuba diving. Those whose studying took place in one location (e.g., on land) and testing took place in a different location (e.g., in water while they were scuba diving) did not perform as well.Later research demonstrated that the change in physical context needs to be fairly dramatic to affect memory. For example, Saufley, Otaka, & Bravaresco (1986) showed that students’ test performance did not change when they were tested in the same classroom they learned in or a different classroom.If students need to recall information (for a test, for example) in a new context that differs greatly from the context in which they learned or studied the information, they should try to imagine the learning context before they start recalling.Go to Activity 4.2-The Importance of Cues (Mantyla cue demonstration)State-dependent memoryMemory is best when a person’s emotional or physiological state is the same when he/she is being tested as it was when he/she was learning.As with context effects, the change in the person’s physiological state needs to be fairly significant to affect memory (e.g., a person’s not drinking a caffeinated soda while testing if the person drank one while learning should not affect the person’s performance much). ................
................

In order to avoid copyright disputes, this page is only a partial summary.

Google Online Preview   Download