PODROČJA: POSAMEZNIK IN TRAVMA



TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK

PREVALENCA TRAVME

In a recent review of studies investigating civilian-related trauma and PTSD, Resnick, Falsetti, Kilpatrick, and Freedy (1995) reported that lifetime exposure to a variety of traumatic events is relatively common (40-70%), with prevalence rates of PTSD ranging from 18% to 28% for individuals exposed to some type of civilian trauma. In the largest study on the prevalence of traumatic events and PTSD conducted to date (The National Cormorbidity Survey), Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes, and Nelson (1995) surveyed a representative U.S. sample of 8,098 individuals 15 to 54 years of age and found that 60% of men and 51% of women reported experiencing at least one traumatic event in their lifetime, with 8% of the sample estimated to have a lifetime diagnosis of PTSD.

Several studies have examined prevalence of traumatic events among college students. These studies report that exposure to traumatic events among college students also is relatively common. Vrana and Lauterbach (1994) found that 84% of college students reported experiencing at least one traumatic event during their lives, and over one third experienced four or more lifetime traumatic events. Green (1995) found a similarly high rate (>90%) of lifetime exposure to at least one traumatic event among members of a college sample. However, in both studies of college students, no attempts were made to examine rates of PTSD, and limited attempts were made to examine predictors of PTSD symptomatology.

The current study is the largest to date to examine the prevalence of lifetime traumatic events and PTSD symptoms in a college sample. These results indicate that having experienced one or more traumatic events is common among college students. Consistent with previous data (e.g., Green, 1995; Vrana & Lauterbach, 1994), approximately 67% of participants reported experiencing at least one traumatic event during their lifetime. Differential reporting rates in the experience of traumatic events were evident, with men more likely to have been in more serious accidents, to have been physically assaulted, and to have witnessed more serious injury or death. Women reported a greater proportion than men of adolescent and adult experiences of sexual coercion and sexual assault. These results are consistent with the findings of Norris (1992), who studied the prevalence of traumatic events among a community sample of adults. She found that women were more likely to have been sexually assaulted, but men were more likely to have been in motor vehicle crashes, to have been nonsexually physically assaulted, or to have experienced some violent event. In the present study, the high prevalence rates of childhood sexual abuse and adolescent and adult sexual assault, particularly among women, are consistent with previous research with college (e.g., Gidycz, Coble, Latham, & Layman, 1993) and community samples (e.g., Finkelhor, Hoatling, Lewis, & Smith, 1990; Kilpatrick & Resnick, 1993).

The finding that a significant proportion of individuals (12% of traumatized respondents; 4% of the full sample) met PTSD criteria within the past week is also important given that this is a college sample. Although it might be argued that this estimate overrepresents the true prevalence of PTSD diagnosis because of the use of self-report methodology, this rate is very similar to estimates obtained in large-scale epidemiological studies that have assessed PTSD diagnosis with structured clinical interviews. For example, Resnick, Kilpatrick, Dansky, Saunders, and Best (1993) studied a national probability sample of 4,008 women, using a comprehensive traumatic event inventory (similar to the one used in this study) and PTSD interview format. The researchers estimated that 7% of women with a history of any type of traumatic event (5% of the full sample) met criteria for current PTSD diagnosis.

In terms of exposure to violence, Jenkins (2001) found in a literature review of selected studies that between 26% and 70% of inner city children have been exposed to severe violence, such as witnessing a shooting.

Bryant-Davis, T. (2005). Coping Strategies of African American Adult Survivors of Childhood Violence. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 36(4), 409–414.

In a study conducted 52 years after the Dresden bombing, we explored pathogenetic and salutogenetic aspects of the psychological consequences of the bombing night trauma in a population now aged between 57 and 95. The assumption guiding the investigation was that, although a traumatic incident like the Dresden bombing has pathological long-term aftereffects, the processing of the traumatic experiences may also contribute to personal growth (cf. Antonovsky, 1987; Frankl, 1973). Whereas pathogenetic factors contribute to the symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), salutogenetic factors are associated with the successful processing of the trauma and a nonsymptomatic and/or a positive outcome in terms of personal growth.

Maercker, A. in Herrle, J. (2003). Long-Term Effects of the Dresden Bombing: Relationships to Control Beliefs, Religious Belief, and Personal Growth. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16(6), 579-587.

Accidents, suicides, and homicides are the three leading

causes of death among young people in the United

States (U.S. Bureau of Census, 1999).

(The Prevalence of PTSD Following the Violent Death of a Child and Predictors of Change 5 Years Later, Shirley A. Murphy,1;3 L. Clark Johnson,1 Ick-Joong Chung,2 and Randal D. Beaton1

Journal of Traumatic Stress, Vol. 16, No. 1, February 2003, pp. 17–25 ( C ° 2003))

PREVALENCA TRAVME – RAZLIKE MED SPOLOMA

Gender is one potentially very powerful risk factor for victimization. In

general, men are more likely to be exposed towar combat, nonsexual assaults

between strangers, and to be victimized in public places (Craven, 1997; U.S.

Census Bureau, 2003), whereas women are more likely to be sexually

abused, injured by an intimate partner, and victimized in a private home (Craven,

1997; Finkelhor, 1994; Straus, 2001).

A recent community survey revealed a number of gender differences in

exposure to various kinds of trauma (Goldberg & Freyd, under review).

Women were much more likely to report having been emotionally or psychologically

mistreated by someone close as adults (approximately 40% compared

to less than 12% of men) and as children (approximately 30% compared

to less than 14%). Women also reported more sexual abuse in

adulthood and in childhood than did men. However, men were much more

likely to report having witnessed someone who they were not close to being

killed, committing suicide, or being injured, in adulthood and childhood.

Overall, women reported more events involving someone close to them, and

men reported more events that did not involve other people, and events

involving others who were not close to them.

These data suggest that victims of betrayal-related events are more likely

to be women than men, whereas victims of nonbetrayal events are more

likely to be men. Exposure to different types of trauma may be one form of

gender-based socialization that affects a range of psychological, social, and

physical health outcomes (DePrince & Freyd, 2002; Freyd, 1999).

Boys and girls reported anger as their primary reason for violence, however girls were more likely to report using violence as selfdefense, whereas boys reported using violence to exert control over their

dating partner.

DATING VIOLENCE AMONG ADOLESCENTS Prevalence, Gender Distribution, and Prevention Program Effectiveness

LAURA J. HICKMAN, LISA H. JAYCOX, RAND Corporation. JESSICA ARONOFF,

Break the Cycle

TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 5, No. 2, April 2004 123-142

More boys reported perpetrating sexual abuse than girls (37% and 24%, respectively) and

more girls reported perpetrating physical abuse than boys (28% and 11%, respectively).

DATING VIOLENCE AMONG ADOLESCENTS Prevalence, Gender Distribution, and Prevention Program Effectiveness

LAURA J. HICKMAN, LISA H. JAYCOX, RAND Corporation. JESSICA ARONOFF, Break the Cycle

TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 5, No. 2, April 2004 123-142

PREVALENCA TRAVME – SPOLNA ZLORABA IN POSILSTVO

Women in the CSA group experienced a first episode of abuse at a mean

age of 8.8 years (SD = 3.9); Table 2 describes ages and types of abuse. More

than one third of the women were victims of two or more perpetrators, and

58% were victims of incest at some point in their childhood. The majority of

women were first abused by an unrelated male, a category that includes people

who were familiar to the victim (e.g., mother’s boyfriend) and people

who were unfamiliar to the victim (e.g., a stranger). Most victims were 15 or

more years younger than their perpetrator. The duration of abuse was generally

either a single incident or more than 2 years, and nearly one half of the

women experienced penetration.

Gorey and Leslie (1997) reviewed surveys involving North American community samples and estimated that 15% of women and 7% of men had experienced contact sexual abuse when they were children. Rates in Europe, Latin America, Africa, Australia, and New Zealand appear comparable to those in North America (Fergusson, Lynskey, &Horwood, 1996a; Finkelhor, 1994).

More than one half of all respondents reported having been previously sexually assaulted (see Humphrey & White, 2000), with most victims being assaulted by people they knew, providing additional evidence to discount the stereotype of sexual assault being commonly committed by so called strangers (Crime Victims Research Treatment Center, 1992; Fieldhaus, Houry, &

Kaminsky, 2000).

Many women experience sexual assault at some time in their lives. Prevalence studies suggest that one third of women (32% to 34%) have experiencedchildhood sexual abuse (CSA; Vogeltanz et al., 1999; Wyatt, Loeb, Solis, Carmona, & Romero, 1999), whereas the prevalence of rape in adult women ranges from 14% to 25% (Koss, 1993). Humphrey and White (2000) found that 50% of college-bound women experience sexual assault prior to college, with 24% to 31% reporting sexual assault during each year of college. Women with victimization histories are at elevated risk of revictimization. Across several studies, 44% of survivors of CSA experienced adolescent or adult sexual assault (Breitenbecher, 2001). Merrill et al. (1999) found that survivors of CSA are 4.8 times as likely to experience adult victimization, and Humphrey and White (2000) found that women who experienced adolescent victimization were 14 times as likely to experience victimization during college.

The Rape Abuse and Incest National Network (1998) reported that one woman is raped every 2 minutes in the United States.

Estimates of the prevalence of child sexual abuse (CSA) in the general female population range from 15% to 33% (for review, see Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993). Approximately 20% of women are victims of rape each year (Russell, 1983; Wyatt, 1985), and it is estimated that between 25% (Straus & Gelles, 1990) and 50% (Stark & Flitcraft, 1988) of women are physically abused by their husbands. The Revictimization of Child Sexual Abuse Survivors: An Examination of the Adjustment of College Women With Child Sexual Abuse, Adult Sexual Assault, and Adult Physical Abuse , CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2000)

Partner abuse against women is a serious public health problem in the United States. Approximately 4.4 million women are estimated to suffer from partner abuse each year (Misra, 2001).

Russell’s (1983, 1984) landmark study of community women in San Francisco

revealed that 24% of women had experienced a completed rape and

44% had experienced a completed or attempted rape. Koss and her colleagues

conducted a national random survey of college women and found

that 1 in 4 women had experienced rape or attempted rape in their lifetimes

and 84% of the women knew their attacker (Koss, Gidycz, & Wisniewski,

1987). Rape was not rare, and it was not primarily a stranger-in-the-bushes

phenomenon. Itwas a violent crime committed against millions ofwomen by

men they knewand trusted.

(REBECCA CAMPBELL, SHARON M. WASCO, Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131)

Over the past two decades, the study of CSAwithin the family

and its sequelae has been recognized as a significant issue for

many women. Estimates vary as to its prevalence, ranging from

about one third (31%) (Russell, 1983) (33%) (Wheeler & Walton,

1987) to 16% (Russell, 1983, 1984; Sedney & Brooks, 1984)

Duration. The average duration of the CSA for the mutilators

was almost double that of the nonmutilators. With a range of

duration from less than 1 year to 22 years, the mutilators’ CSA

began at the mean age of 6.06 years (SD = 3.91) and stopped at the

mean age of 13.33 years (SD = 5.87). For the nonmutilators, the

CSA started at the mean age of 7.64 years (SD = 4.30), and the

mean age when it ended was 11.66 (SD = 6.36). Duration ranged

from less than 1 year to 34 years. Duration of the CSAappeared to

differentiate between the two groups and was included as a

potential variable in the model-building phase of the analysis.

Childhood sexual abuse (CSA) occurs in the lives of about 1 in 4 girls

(Finkelhor, Hotaling, Lewis,&Smith, 1990; Russell, 1983).

Thirteen percent of the 4,008 women surveyed in the National Women’s

Study (National Victim Center and Crime Victims Research and Treatment

Center, 1992) reported having experienced at least one completed, forcible

rape in their lifetime. The investigators estimate that 638,000 American

women were raped in the year before the survey was conducted. Rape was

defined in the study’s preface as “an event that occurred without the

woman’s consent, involved the use of force or threat of force, and involved

sexual penetration of the victim’s vagina, mouth, or rectum.” Sexual assaults

that did not involve force, threat of force, or penetration were not included in

the prevalence and incidence estimates. (Women’s Responses to Sexual Violence by Male Intimates

Claire Burke Draucker Phyllis Noerager Stern Western Journal of Nursing Research, 2000, 22(4), 385-406)

Russell’s (1983, 1984) landmark study of community women in San Francisco

revealed that 24% of women had experienced a completed rape and

44% had experienced a completed or attempted rape. Koss and her colleagues

conducted a national random survey of college women and found

that 1 in 4 women had experienced rape or attempted rape in their lifetimes

and 84% of the women knew their attacker (Koss, Gidycz, & Wisniewski,

1987). Rape was not rare, and it was not primarily a stranger-in-the-bushes

phenomenon. Itwas a violent crime committed against millions ofwomen by

men they knewand trusted. Comparable prevalence rates have been obtained

by multiple independent research teams, and 20 years later it is still clear that

sexual assault is far too prevalent. (Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault 20 Years of Progress and Future Directions REBECCA CAMPBELL,SHARON M. WASCO

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131)

Despite prevalence estimates that 15% of males in the United States and

Canada have a history of child sexual abuse (CSA) (Bagley,Wood,&Young,

1994; Finkelhor, Hotaling, Lewis, & Smith, 1990), research on male victims

of CSA continues to lag behind that research on female victims (Kendall-

Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993).

Definitions of masculinity tend not to allow expression of the fear, vul-

nerability, and helplessness that accompany the experience of sexual abuse

(Dimock, 1988; Hunter, 1991; Lisak, 1994; Nasjleti, 1980).

Agrowing body of literature has documented that the sequelae of CSA for

male victims are much the same as for female victims (see reviews in

Finkelhor, 1990; Urquiza & Capra, 1990).

Data from the National Child Abuse and Neglect Data System indicate that

2.8 per 1,000women experienced childhood physical abuse and 2.3 per 1,000

experienced childhood sexual abuse in 1998 (United States Department of

Health and Human Services, 2000). In terms of prevalence, national data

reveal that approximately one and one half million children have experienced

physical abuse (Straus & Gelles, 1990) and almost one half million children

are sexually abused prior to age 18 (Sedlak, 1991).

ALTHOUGHTHETRUEPREVALENCEof sexual abuse is unknown, research suggests that a significant proportion ofwomenin today’s society have experienced this trauma (e.g., Ganley, 1981, 1989; Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans, & Herbison, 1993; Pence & Paymar, 1986). In general medical practice, sex therapy, and institutional psychiatry, it is estimated that 25%, 50%, and 51% of women, respectively, report a history of sexual abuse (Craine, Hensen, Colliver, & MacLean, 1988).

PREVALENCA TRAVME – DOMAČE NASILJE

Domestic violence has reached epidemic proportions in the United States.

Each year at least 4 million women are victimized, 2 million suffer serious

injury, and 3,000 women suffer fatal injuries (Tjaden & Thoennes, 2000).

Underscoring the seriousness of domestic violence is that 30% of women

killed in the United States die at the hands of a husband or boyfriend (Russell,

1995). Studies estimate that between 33% and 50% of women in the general

population are physically abused by their husbands, ex-husbands, or live-in

partners during their lifetime (Canadian Abilities Foundation, 2002; Tjaden

& Thoennes, 2000). Annually, approximately 1.5%, or 4.8 million, women

are raped or physically assaulted by an intimate partner in the United States

(Tjaden & Thoennes, 2000).

(MRUGAYA W. GORDE, CHRISTINE A. HELFRICH, MARCIA L. FINLAYSON Trauma Symptoms and Life Skill Needs of Domestic Violence Victims, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 19 No. 6, June 2004 691-708)

Violence against women and children is a serious problem in this country.

Estimates of the prevalence of child sexual abuse (CSA) in the general female

population range from 15% to 33% (Kendall-Tackett,Williams,&Finkelhor,

1993). The national crime victim survey found that one out of eight women

are victims of rape during their lifetime (National Victim Center, 1992), and

Koss (1993) estimated rape and sexual assault prevalence among adult

women to be between 15% and 25%. Approximately 25% (Straus & Gelles, 1990) to 50% (Stark & Flitcraft, 1988) of women are physically battered by

their husbands. Women who have been raped or battered may experience a

wide range of effects that may manifest in symptoms of depression, anxiety,

posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and sexual dysfunction (Goodman,

Koss,&Russo, 1993). The psychological sequelae ofCSAare similar and may

persist into adulthood (Kendall-Tackett et al., 1993; Polusny&Follette, 1995).

Moreover, in a recently studied community sample of

close to 10,000 individuals, more than 26% reported having been victims

of childhood physical violence, whereas only approximately 8%

reported having experienced childhood sexual abuse (MacMillan

et al., 1997).

PREVALENCA TRAVME – ZANEMARJANJE

However, as has been

noted frequently, research on neglect is only a small

fraction of research on child maltreatment (National

Research Council, 1993). Cross-cultural comparative

studies of neglect are even more rare.

There are grounds for believing that neglect is as

detrimental or more detrimental than physical or sexual

abuse. Neglect, especially of the child’s emotional

needs for love and support, may be the form of maltreatment

with the greatest risk of serious social and

psychological problems to children (Bowlby, 1982;

Robbins, 1966; Spitz, 1959).

PREVALENCA TRAVME – SMRT BLIŽNJEGA

However, due to the limited

availability of national data, it remains unclear how prevalent deaths of

family members and friends are among adolescents.

Perhaps most important, prevalence data for the current study oˇer

information on experience of loss in a nationally representative sample of

adolescents.

Results of this study indicate that adolescents’ exposure to death is high,

with one in three adolescents reporting the death of a family member in the

past year and one in ˘ve adolescents reporting the past-year death of a close

friend. Several demographic factorswere associated with higher prevalence of

past-year death of a familymember.Girlsweremore likely thanboys to report

experiencing the death of a family member in the past year. In addition,

adolescents with lower household incomes were more likely than adolescents

with higher household incomes to report the past-year death of a family

member.

PREDIKTORJI

PREDIKTORJI – SPOLNA ZLORABA, POSILSTVO

Rape victims with a history of child sexual abuse were found to have higher levels of

trauma symptoms, made greater use of nervous and cognitive coping strategies, and were more likely to make attributions of blame towards themselves or society. Current symptoms were related to types of coping and attributions of blame, with history of child sexual abuse having an indirect relationship to these variables. The results suggest the importance of attributional and coping variables, as well as child sexual abuse history, as mediators of postrape adjustment.

Sex role socialization theory proposes that there are developmental

processes by which individuals learn what is appropriate for their

gender. Through these processes, both women and men form expectations

concerning acceptable behaviors in sexual interactions (Bridges, 1991).

According to this theory, those who adhere to extremely traditional sex role

socialization beliefs may viewacquaintance rape as an extreme and appropriate

version of male-female sexual interactions. In general, the theory holds

that the process of sex role socialization promotes the formation of rape-supportive

beliefs, or false beliefs about rape, that can serve to mitigate the seriousness

of rape. Rape-supportive beliefswould include beliefs such as rape is

not psychologically damaging to victims as well as stereotypical beliefs

about the victim’s role in her sexual assault (e.g., victim’s control over,

enjoyment of, and responsibility for her assault).

Results of the current study revealed that individuals made significantly

less rape-supportive attributions when the victim-perpetrator relationship was

dissolving than when the relationship was intact. Consistent with these findings,

several states more fully support the prosecution of husband perpetrators

if the couple is separated, living apart, seeking divorce, or divorced

(Augustine, 1991). The legal community appears to consider sexual assaults

that occur within dissolving marital relationships to more closely meet the

nonconsensual criteria for rape. However, contrary to expectation, attributions

regarding sex role stereotypical victim blame were not found to differ

among the relationship conditions. This finding may be an indication that there

has been a reduction in the societal tendency to blame the married victim.

Adult rape victims with a history of child sexual abuse are reported to

have longer recovery times (Burgess&Holmstrom, 1978) and poorer global

social adjustment (Frank, Turner, & Stewart, 1980). In addition, repetitive

victims have been found to have higher initial levels of distress and longer recovery

times from the most recent episode when compared to first-time victims

(Ruch, Arnedeo, Leon, & Gartrell, 1991).

Research on adult women with histories of child sexual abuse have also

documented the importance of self-blame and coping variables as mediators

of adjustment.

Why does childhood sexual abuse have a different impact on different individuals? One of the most supported

explanations reported in etiological theories and studies of sexual abuse

suggests that some variables may have an effect on the impact of childhood sexual

abuse on victims’ development. Such variables could be organized along two

principal axes: (a) “sexual abuse factors,” such as the variables related to the sexual

contacts per se (nature, duration, frequency), those relative to the adult perpetrator

(gender, age, tie to child), and those regarding the child victim (gender, age,

emotions felt at time of contacts, dysfunctional, negligent, or violent family setting);

(b) “later” or “iatrogenic factors,” such as the reaction of family and friends

to the disclosure, whether the case is in the hands of the judicial or medical

system, and number of interveners.

According to the literature, the use of violence versus its absence during sexual

abuse have an effect on victims’ feelings and perception with regard to the abuse.

When sexual abuse is accompanied by physical force or verbal threats, victims of

such contacts have reported feeling manipulated, betrayed, and humiliated. They

have also indicated experiencing a profound sense of distress, relational difficulties

(lack of trust in others and excessive wariness), poor self-esteem, feelings of

guilt and helplessness (Doll et al., 1992; Gartner, 1999; Haugaard&Emery, 1989;

Stein, Jacobs, Ferguson, Allen, & Fonagy, 1998; Urquiza & Capra, 1990), and

sexual problems in adulthood (Mendel, 1995; T. G. M. Sandfort, 1992).

In contrast, when sexual abuse occurred in a context

of subtle manipulation and exempt of violence, certain victims have reported

being consenting at the time of these experiences and even having initiated them

(Okami, 1991; T. G. Sandfort, 1984). These victims qualified these events as positive

experiences, discoveries, and sources of physical and sexual pleasure

(Okami, 1991). In other words, it appears that the child’s consent at the time of

such sexual contacts with an adult and the absence of force or threats are related to

the victim’s positive perception. In this regard, T. G. M. Sandfort (1992) reported

that victims who had nonconsensual contacts were more likely to present with

poor adaptation and sexual dissatisfaction in adulthood than were those who

reported consensual contacts.

Where victims’ emotions at time of abuse are concerned, most studies have

pointed out the existence of links between the presence of negative emotions associated

with sexual abuse and the development of feelings of guilt, shame, and

betrayal, which brought about depression and self-destructive behaviour over the

medium and long term (Finkelhor & Browne, 1985; Gartner, 1999; Hauggard &

Emery, 1989; Newberger & DeVos, 1988; Okami, 1991; Stein et al., 1998).

Meanwhile, according to some etiological theories and studies, the symptoms

observed among certain victims are more the result of the victim’s familial problems

and dysfunctions (parental alcoholism, physical violence, negligence) than

of the sexual contacts per se (Berliner, 1991; Hansen, Hecht, & Futa, 1998).

In this regard, when families failed to respond to the needs

and distress of victims, the latter have reported being as much disturbed by this

absence of family support as by the sexual abuse per se (Bernard, 1981; Browne&

Finkelhor, 1986; Faller, 1993; Finkelhor, 1990; Ingram, 1981; Kendall-Tackett,

Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993; Rind & Bauserman, 1993; Sauzier, 1989). Similarly,

children who did not enjoy a reassuring family or structured social support

(e.g., school, therapy) have been found to be at higher risk of developing psychological

or behavioural problems in adulthood (Falshaw, Browne, &Hollin, 1996;

Kendall-Tackett et al., 1993). However, it has also been reported that invasive

interventions could be just as harmful as the absence of family and structured support

to the victim’s development (Elwell & Ephross, 1987; Van Gijseghem,

1998).

Furthermore, it is important to note that certain variables have not been studied

much with male samples despite having proved crucial in the psychosocial adjustment

ofwomen victims of childhood sexual abuse. Among these variables, the literature

points out the importance of the family constellation and of the victim’s

attachment style (Alexander, 1992; Alexander & Lupfer, 1987; Finkelhor, 1990;

Harter, Alexander,&Neimeyer, 1988; Levang, 1989; Zeanah&Zeanah, 1989).

The results of the present study suggest that the investigation of the impact of

childhood sexual abuse requires that a multitude of variables be considered at the

same time. These include variables related to the sexual abuse scenario, family

context, attachment style, and possibly, to other types of victimization (negli-

gence and verbal, physical, or psychological abuse) and to the circumstances of

disclosure, which were not included in our analyses on account of their low frequency

in our sample. Furthermore, the results suggest that similar childhood

experiences do not elicit one and the same reaction in adulthood. Moreover, similar

adjustment problems may arise from different sources. Finally, it does not

seem possible to establish an exclusive causal link between childhood sexual

abuse and an individual’s psychosocial adjustment in adulthood.

In the majority of previous studies, abuse-related factors (e.g., proximity to the abuser, frequency and duration of the abuse, severity of the acts) were not found to be predictive of children’s or adolescents’adjustment following sexual abuse (Dubowitz, Black, Harrington, &Verschoore, 1993; Manion et al., 1998; Mannarino et al., 1991; Tebbutt et al., 1997). Some evidence was found for the predictive value of negative appraisals, symptomatology at initial assessment, and social support. In their study on 56 sexually abused children and adolescents, Manion et al. (1998) found that negative appraisals of guilt and blame predicted emotional functioning at 12 months postdisclosure. Also, and consistent with the results of Freedman, Brandes, Peri, and Shalev (1998), the authors show that internalizing symptoms, such as dissociation, depression and anxiety, and 3-month postdisclosure, are associated with more symptomatology at 12 months postdisclosure. They suggest that early internalizing symptoms and their concomitant negative appraisals of the traumatic event may reduce the victim’s ability to recover (Freedman et al., 1998). Social support also has a predictive value in recovery after sexual abuse (seeKendall-Tackett et al., 1993).

Lynskey and Fergusson (1997) identified factors that discriminated sexually abused youngsters who developed a psychiatric disorder or adjustment difficulties from sexually abused youngsters who did not develop such problems. Results showed that parental support was an important factor protecting against the development of adjustment difficulties.With increasing reports of support, affection, and nurture, the occurrence of later adjustment difficulties decreased. This is consistent with Joseph (1999) who argued that crisis support immediately after the traumatic event is influential on later functioning. In his study on adolescent survivors of a ship disaster, greater direct crisis support was predictive of fewer feelings of depression and anxiety 18 months later.

While numerous risk factors associated with the development of PTSD symptoms have been studied, there is still considerable confusion about why some individuals develop PTSD, and others remain relatively unscathed. Risk factors for the development and maintenance of PTSD symptoms include general vulnerability and severity of trauma exposure. Vulnerability factors related to PTSD include female gender, with women more likely to develop PTSD than men (Breslau et al., 1991; Norris, 1992), and history of exposure to traumatic events (Kilpatrick, Resnick, Saunders, & Best, 1998; Vrana & Lauterbach, 1994). Trauma severity indices associated with PTSD include threat to life, injury, and witnessing serious injury or death of another person. These factors indicate the degree of threat posed to an individual and are associated with the development of PTSD (e.g., Green, 1990; Kilpatrick et al., 1989; Kilpatrick & Resnick, 1993).

More recent research has suggested that individual response characteristics are also important predictors of PTSD symptoms. Specifically, peritraumatic reactions, immediate reactions experienced at the time of the trauma, such as dissociation, extreme anxiety, panic, and/or negative emotions, may be important predictors of subsequent PTSD symptoms. One reaction at the time of the trauma that has received considerable attention is dissociation. Marmar and his colleagues, have shown that peritraumatic dissociation is predictive of PTSD symptoms over and above the contribution of level of stress exposure and general dissociative tendencies in both male (Marmar et al., 1994) and female Vietnam veterans (Tichenor, Marmar, Weiss, Metzler, & Ronfeldt, 1996). The finding that peritraumatic dissociation is a robust predictor of PTSD symptoms has been found in other trauma populations as well. Weiss and colleagues (Weiss, Marmar, Metzler, & Ronfeldt, 1995) studied predictors of PTSD symptomatic responses among a group of emergency services personnel. After controlling for demographic variables, severity of exposure to a critical incident, adjustment, social support, locus of control, and general dissociative tendencies, peritraumatic dissociation remained strongly predictive of PTSD symptoms. In a study of the survivors of the Oakland/Berkeley, California firestorm, Koopman, Classen, and Spiegel (1994) found that individual dissociative symptoms occurring immediately after the fire predicted PTSD symptoms 7 to 9 months later. Moreover, in a recent prospective study of 51 injured trauma survivors, Shalev, Peri, Cannetti, and Schreiber (1996) found that peritraumatic dissociation reported 1 week after the trauma explained approximately 30% of the variance in PTSD symptoms at 6-month follow-up, above and beyond the contribution of demographic variables, event severity, and initial symptoms of intrusion, avoidance, depression, and anxiety. Despite the fact that immediate responses other than dissociation have received limited attention, several studies suggest that emotional and physical reactions experienced at the time of the trauma may be important predictors of PTSD symptomatic distress. Resick, Churchill, and Falsetti (1990) examined a range of immediate within-assault cognitive and emotional reactions experienced by rape victims. These authors found that emotional and dissociative reactions during the rape accounted for 46 to 79% of the variance in PTSD symptoms. In another study, Resnick, Falsetti, Kilpatrick, and Foy (1994) examined acute panic and emotional responses among a group of rape victims interviewed within 72 hr postrape during an emergency room postrape exam. Almost all (90%) of the women seen at the emergency room reported having a panic attack at the time of the rape. Longitudinal data further indicated that initial panic symptoms were predictive of PTSD intrusion symptoms at 3-month follow-up (Resnick, 1997). Additionally, Moleman, van der Hart, and van der Kolk (1992) reported a link between panic symptoms and dissociation in women who were undergoing extremely complicated childbirth. During childbirth, these women experienced a progression from initial panic symptoms to dissociation, and the majority of the women subsequently developed full-blown PTSD. From these studies, it appears that peritraumatic fear and attendant physiological arousal may lead to cognitive disruption in the form of peritraumatic dissociation and subsequent PTSD.

The theoretical frameworks of early literature on battered women assumed pathology of the women and focused little or no attention on the consequences of experiencing violent acts (Gelles & Harrop, 1989; Walker & Browne, 1985). Yet research over the past decade indicates that the behaviors battered women demonstrate are primarily the result of the severity of threat and harm experienced (Follingstad, Brennan, Hause, Polek, & Rutledge, 1991; Ochberg, 1991; Walker & Browne, 1985).

Although we did not find evidence that posttraumatic symptomatology is an underlying mechanism through which previous victimization leads to subsequent victimization (i.e., a mediator variable; Baron & Kenny, 1986),

our data do highlight the importance of taking into account current levels of posttraumatic symptomatology when examining the link between child and/or adolescent sexual victimization and subsequent sexual victimization. Individuals with a history of previous sexual victimization who are experiencing PTSD symptoms are likely to have difficulty recognizing, attending to, or responding to danger cues appropriately (van der Kolk & McFarlane,

1996). They might have trouble discriminating threatening from nonthreatening information or make unwise decisions based on incomplete or inaccurate information. Moreover, perpetrators could identify these individuals

as easy targets. As a result, they are at increased risk for sexual revictimization.

ZAŠČITNI FAKTORJI

Persistent unresolved anger or related negative feelings might serve to

promote posttrauma symptoms, whereas forgiveness may be accompanied by less severe symptoms.

Higher levels of anger were strongly associated with health status, emotional distress, and PTSD symptom severity.

A growing body of evidence suggests that the personality trait of hardiness (Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn, 1982) helps to buffer exposure to extreme stress. Hardiness consists of three dimensions: being committed to finding meaningful purpose in life, the belief that one can influence one's surroundings and the outcome of events, and the belief that one can learn and grow from both positive and negative life experiences. Armed with this set of beliefs, hardy individuals have been found to appraise potentially stressful situations as less threatening, thus minimizing the experience of distress. Hardy individuals are also more confident and better able to use active coping and social support, thus helping them deal with the distress they do experience (e. g. , Florian, Mikulincer, & Taubman, 1995).

In an attempt to address this, they suggested

that the negative sequelae of childhood sexual abuse

could be viewed as a form of PTSD.

From studies of adults who were sexually abused in childhood, it was suggested that

individual differences in response to the abuse were related

to three mediating variables including: severity of the abuse,

availability of social support, and attributional styles regarding

the cause of negative life events (Gold, 1986; Seidner and

Calhoun, 1984; Silver et al., 1983). These three variables have

also been shown to act as risk factors for onset of PTSD when

exposed to negative life events such as rape or exposure to war

(Baker and Peterson, 1977; Steketee and Foa, 1987; Cluss

et al., 1983; Foy et al., 1984). Similar mediating factors in

childhood sexual abuse have been presented by Kendall-

Tackett et al. (1993).

Finkelhor and Berliner (1995) report that research has

clearly demonstrated that certain elements are consistent predictors

of the level of distress in children and the speed of their

recovery. These influencing factors include parental support

(Everson et al., 1991), maternal upset (Deblinger et al., 1990;

Newberger et al., 1993), help-seeking in response to family

crisis (Waterman, 1993) and also general elements of family

functioning such as cohesion and healthy conflict management

(Conte and Schuerman, 1987).

The findings support the assumption that people

who felt adequately informed would show better psychological adjustment.

One should note that this aspect of cognitive coping at the first

assessment (1989) had the strongest relationship with later symptoms.

These findings suggest that assisting traumatized persons in their

search for verifiable information may be important in early intervention

efforts.

The hypothesis that viewing the accident as

a random incident would be associated with better psychological adjustment

was supported.

Whether antisocial youth become violent

adults is determined by multiple risk and protective factors (Rutter,

Giller, & Hagell, 1998). Such factors include individual strengths and vulnerabilities,

family characteristics, and features of the wider community

(including the peer group, school, and neighborhood). Learning difficulties,

difficult temperament, and problems with regulating negative emotions are

examples of individual risk factors, whereas easy temperament and good

problem-solving skills are examples of individual protective factors. Examples

of family-based risk factors include insecure attachment, family violence,

and family disorganization, whereas examples of protective family

factors include secure attachment, parental cooperation, and effective discipline

practices. Membership of deviant peer groups and a low level of family

support are examples of community-based risk factors. In contrast, involvement

with nondeviant peers and a high level of family support are examples

of protective factors.

This study suggests that not all sexually abused children have the same perceived level of self-concept, social support, or traumatic symptoms. This indicates that there are variations to sexually abused childrens' response to the trauma and supports the examination of individual differences among this heterogeneous population. Perceived parental, classmate, and friend support were associated with higher global self-concept. This suggests that sexually abused children who felt better about themselves also perceived a higher level of social support. Children who reported high levels of posttraumatic stress indicated feeling worse about their behavior; children who reported high levels of dissociation reported more negative feelings about their social acceptance. The stability of these relationships will be reexamined with the larger sample of children when data are available.

Sexually Abused Children's Perceptions: How They May Change Treatment Focus ,  By: Carla J. Reyes, Anna M. Kokotovic, Merith A. Cosden, Professional Psychology: Research And Practice, 0735-7028, December 1, 1996, Vol. 27, Issue 6

One important element

of coping identified by Joseph et al. (1995) is crisis support. Crisis support

has been defined by Joseph et al. (1995) as involving both the availability of

others and their reactions to disclosures of trauma. The evidence suggests

that, in general, individuals who receive higher levels of crisis support experience

better psychological outcome (Joseph, Andrews, Williams, & Yule,

1992). Joseph et al.’s (1995) model indicates that crisis support is thought to

influence appraisals, coping, and emotional states.

A Cognitive-Behavioral Model of Post-Traumatic Stress for Sexually Abused Females -

SUZANNE L. BARKER-COLLO, WILLIAM T. MELNYK, LESLIE MCDONALD-MISZCZAK -

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 15 No. 4, April 2000 375-392

In their discussion,

the authors noted general agreement in the literature (Jones & Barlow,

1990; Joseph et al., 1996; S. D. Solomon, 1986) that increased availability of

crisis support is predictive of external event appraisals (i.e., attribution of

blame to others) and reduced PTSD symptomatology. However, some traumatic

events (e.g., sexual abuse) can be stigmatizing and elicit shunning,

avoidance, and blaming of the victim by crisis supports (Wortman & Lehman,

1985). Shunning, avoidance, and blaming by the support network and

failure to engage the network may be particularly evident where the abuser is

known to the survivor due to increased efforts to deny or hide the occurrence

of the abuse (Meichenbaum, 1994).

A Cognitive-Behavioral Model of Post-Traumatic Stress for Sexually Abused Females -

SUZANNE L. BARKER-COLLO, WILLIAM T. MELNYK, LESLIE MCDONALD-MISZCZAK -

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 15 No. 4, April 2000 375-392

FAKTORJI TVEGANJA

As expected, people with increased exposure and higher identification with the victims

encountered higher stress (Schuster et al., 2001).

In particular, evidence suggests that individuals who are vulnerable to faulty regulation of negative emotion may be at increased risk for aggressive and/or violent behavior (Davidson, Putnam, & Larson, 2000).

Although age is clearly an important factor in placing girls and young

women at risk for sexual assault, life course stage is also likely to shape the

mental health consequences of those violent events. Adolescence is a period

of self-exploration, a stage in which girls acquire a sense of self-worth and

when self-esteem develops and becomes more stable (Steinberg & Sheffield

Morris, 2001). Given that adolescence is a period in the life course that

requires adjustment to a number of major social, cognitive, and psychological

changes (Lerner & Galambos, 1999), exposure to violence may disrupt

children’s progression through age-appropriate developmental tasks

(Margolin & Gordis, 2000). Egan and Perry (1998) suggested that poor selfconceptions

are risk factors and consequences of victimization during adolescence,

diminishing self-regard over time and solidifying a child’s status as

a victim.Violent victimization during this time period may, therefore, be particularly

consequential. Experiences in adolescence will not only shape the

direction the life course will take but also will, in large part, determine theavailability and stability of adult social roles (Clausen, 1991). Although

Loeber and Hay (1997) pointed out that early stressful life events may lead to

a variety of consequences including the development of negative behaviors,

Recent nation wide studies established that the major predictors of post-traumatic stress disorder

were the objective severity of the violence inflicted, the subjective fear of death or serious injury,

and whether penetration of the body occurred (Epstein, Saunders, & Kilpatrick,1997). Also important were how much awoman blamed herself for what happened and how threatening the rape was to her worldview (Frazier,1990; Frazier & Schauben, 1994; Koss, Figueredo, Prince,&White, 2000; Norris&Kaniasty, 1991).

A number of studies have shown that trauma symptoms are related to the

severity of stressful life events. This has been found in research on various

kinds of traumatic events, including among victims of motor vehicle accidents

(Ehlers, Mayou, & Bryant, 1998), Vietnam veterans (Green, Grace,

Lindy, Gleser, & Leonard, 1990), Cambodian refugees (Carlson & Rosser-

Hogan, 1991), and survivors of a firestorm (Koopman, Classen, & Spiegel,

1994). (Recent Stressful Life Events,Sexual Revictimization, and Their

Relationship With Traumatic Stress Symptoms Among Women Sexually Abused in ChildhoodCATHERINE CLASSEN

RUTH NEVO,CHERYL KOOPMAN,KIRSTEN NEVILL-MANNING,CHERYL GORE-FELTON

DEBORAH S. ROSE,DAVID SPIEGEL

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 12, December 2002 1274-1290)

Tveganje za spolno zlorabo in navezanost

Children with insecure patent-child relationships,

aswehave seen, will have low self-esteem,

poor relationship skills, and a desperate need

for attention. As a consequence, they will readily

respond to attention from any adult. Being unable

to form relationships themselves and lacking

confidence that anyone will like them, these

children will be especially vulnerable to attention

fromothers. They can be expected to be particularly

responsive to physical attention.

However, children who experienced anxious/

ambivalent relations with their parents will be

more likely to be responsive to attention from

others because they have a positive view of others

and strongly desire closeness. Avoidant children

do not trust others and may be repelled by

adults who display physical affection. Thus,

any adult who pays attention to an anxious/

ambivalent child can expect to get a strongly

positive response. In fact, such a vulnerable child

may bewilling to tolerate even sexual advances

in exchange for feeling close to an adult. Thus,

anxious/ambivalent children may be more

likely to be sexually abused.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

Specifically,

engagement of crisis support has been linked to amount of force used by the

perpetrator.As indicated byWyatt et al. (1991), increasing level of force used

by a perpetrator is significantly related to increasingly negative reactions of

others to the victim when sexual abuse is disclosed.

A Cognitive-Behavioral Model of Post-Traumatic Stress for Sexually Abused Females -

SUZANNE L. BARKER-COLLO, WILLIAM T. MELNYK, LESLIE MCDONALD-MISZCZAK -

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 15 No. 4, April 2000 375-392

clinical and research data suggest that prior traumatic experiences, such as

prior sexual victimization, predict greater psychological distress following

sexual assault (Burgess & Holmstrom, 1978; Frank, Turner, & Stewart,

1980). Adult rape victims with a history of child sexual abuse are reported to

have longer recovery times (Burgess&Holmstrom, 1978) and poorer global

social adjustment (Frank, Turner, & Stewart, 1980). In addition, repetitive

victims have been found to have higher initial levels of distress and longer recovery

times from the most recent episode when compared to first-time victims

(Ruch, Arnedeo, Leon, & Gartrell, 1991).

Coping With Rape The Roles of Prior Sexual Abuse and Attributions of Blame - CATALINA M. ARATA - University of South Alabama - JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 1, January 1999 62-78

Kako izolacija od emocij in relacij privede do nasilja

They propose that men are particularly socialized to suppress this emotion: the sense of being weak, inadequate, powerless, helpless, impotent, or incompetent. Rather than experience these painful feelings or let others see them undergoing them, men usually become blank or angry. Shame itself is harmless, indeed, necessary. Shame is a prime component of conscience, modesty, and morality. It becomes a problem only if covered. That is, one ingredient of violence, its incredible energy, is produced by masking shame with blankness or anger.

Dva odziva na sram

However, patients had two different, seemingly opposite responses in the shame context.

• In one, the patient seemed to be suffering psychological pain, but failed to identify it as shame. Lewis called this form overt, undifferentiated shame. A patient would usually refer to an emotion or feeling, but the reference misidentified the shame feeling (‘This is an awkward moment for me.’)

• In a second type of response, the patient seemed not to be in pain, revealing an emotional response only by rapid, obsessional speech on topics that seemed slightly removed from the dialogue. Lewis called this second response bypassed shame.

Razlika moški ženske

In her study of differences in the way men and women manage emotions, Lewis (1976) cites studies suggesting that the overt, undifferentiated form of unacknowledged shame is more characteristic of women than of men, and the bypassed form more characteristic of men than of women. She uses this difference in the management of shame to explain the higher

rates of depression in women than in men, and the higher level of aggression in men.

Trije vzroki za agresijo in nasilje

• The first is social: isolation, the absence of affectional attachments.

• The second is cognitive: obsessive preoccupation.

• The third is emotional: complete repression of shame in the form of shame/anger spirals.

Zanka ponavljajočih se emocionalnih epizod

Although not stated explicitly by Tomkins or Lewis, both seem to imply that emotions can form closed loops, a self-perpetuating emotional episode that refuses to subside. A familiar example are people who are ‘blushers.’ They are so self-conscious about their blushing that they are ashamed of it. But their shame about blushing increases the blush, and so on. This particular example suggests a loop that is not mentioned by either Tomkins or Lewis: shame/shame. But it is this loop, I believe, that gives rise to the most prevalent form of shame spirals, those that lead to blankness and withdrawal. The two kinds of shame spirals give rise to two different paths: withdrawal and silence (shame/shame) and anger, aggression and violence (shame/anger).

»Emotional/relational« teorija nasilja

The emotional/relational theory of violence outlined here would seem to be particularly applicable to instances involving long-term violence on a massive scale. suggest the three conditions for violence suggested by the theory outlined here: isolation from others, a single, overarching obsession, and complete repression of shame.

MEDIATOR ZA POSLEDICE PRI SPOLNI ZLORABI – NAVEZANOST

Oneway that attachment difficulties might be manifested in later relationships

is through the development of the self. The development of the self can

be seen to unfold in the context of attachment and the internalization of

important others’ perceptions and expectations; sustained and early trauma

arising from abuse can produce long-standing dysfunctions of self (Briere,

1992). Exactly how the “self” should be defined has not been clearly demonstrated,

even by object relations and self psychology theorists for whom it is

central (Briere, 1992). In general, the self can be understood as “the agent of

actions, the experiencer of feelings, the maker of intentions, [and] the architect

of plans” (Stern, 1985, p. 6), the development of which occurs in the context

of attachment. Self-dysfunctions, or those related to this internal base,

are purported to lead to difficulties such as identity confusion, boundary

issues, and the inability to soothe oneself. Attachment theory (Ainsworth,

1985; Bowlby, 1973, 1980, 1982, 1988) suggests that early childhood experiences

of parental support, nurturance, consistency, and responsiveness produce

a secure attachment.Warm and responsive parenting, according to this

model, is expected to result in positive models of both the self and others and

hence to result in secure and fulfilling adult relationships.

Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER - JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207

TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK – RAZVOJ NEVARNE NAVEZANOSTI BOLJ POVEZAN Z ZLORABAMI IN ZANEMARJANJEM

Insecure attachment has been observed to a much greater degree among

children who have experienced physical abuse and neglect than in cases

where abuse and neglect are absent (Carlson, Cicchetti, Barnett, & Braunwald,

1989; Egeland & Sroufe, 1981). It is estimated that between 70% and

100% of maltreated children exhibit insecure attachment (versus a base rate

of about 30% in general population samples) and that these children are more

likely to demonstrate an impaired sense of self and an impaired ability to

share information about their thoughts, feelings, and intentions (Cicchetti,

1987). Similarly, insecure attachment has been noted via clinical observations

of sexually abused children (Friedrich, 1990, 1996) and in research conducted

with adult women. For example, a much higher proportion of insecurely

attached women was found in a group of women who were sexually

abused within their families than the proportion that would be expected

according to Bartholomew and Horowitz’s (1991) normative sample (Alexander,

1993).

Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER - JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207

TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK – NAVEZANOST – +/- MODEL SEBE/DRUGEGA

Patterns of adult attachment, or ways of being in relationships, can be

organized in terms of Bowlby’s (1982) conception of internal working models

(Bartholomew, 1990, 1993). As Bartholomew describes them, models of

the self can be dichotomized as either positive (positive self-concept, the self

as worthy of love and attention) or negative (negative self-concept, the self as

unworthy of love and attention). Similarly, models of the other can be viewed

as positive (the other as trustworthy, caring, and available) or negative (the

other as rejecting, uncaring, and distant (see Figure 1). The degree of positivity

of one’s self-model is associated with the degree of emotional dependence

on others for self-validation; a positive self-model can be understood as an

internalized sense of self-worth that is not dependent on others for validation.

Apositive other-model is reflective of expectations of others’availability and

supportiveness; a positive other-model facilitates actively seeking out intimacy

and support in close relationships, whereas negative other-models lead

to avoidance of intimacy and support (Bartholomew, 1990). Each working

model of the self in combination with each working model of the other is

hypothesized to define a particular adult attachment style (Secure, Fearful,

Dismissing, and Preoccupied; see Figure 1).

[pic]

Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER - JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207

TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK – NAVEZANOST – POJASNILO ODNOSA MED OTROŠKO SPOLNO ZLORABO IN ODRASLO PSIHOLOŠKO PRILAGODITVIJO

In this examination of the relationship between child sexual abuse, adult

attachment style, and adult psychological adjustment, we have demonstrated

that attachment appears to mediate the relationship between CSA and psychological

adjustment. Specifically, we found that CSA predicts both adult

attachment style and psychological adjustment and that attachment also predicts

psychological adjustment. In addition, attachment style continues to

predict adjustment when the effects of CSA are controlled, whereas CSA no

longer predicts adjustment when the effects of attachment are controlled,

thus indicating that adult attachment style mediates the relationship between

CSA and psychological adjustment.

Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER - JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207

TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK – OTROŠKA SPOLNA ZLORABA – OBLIKA NAVEZANOSTI NAPOVEDUJE SIMPTOME PTSM

Because

sexual abuse is strongly associated with adult attachment style, the role of

attachment in adjustment is particularly salient for sexually abused women.

Contrary to Alexander’s (1993) suggestion that attachment predicts basic

personality structure but not symptoms associated with post-traumatic stress

disorder (such as intrusive thoughts, avoidance, and depression), the present

study indicates that attachment is of central importance in predicting these

symptoms. In particular, the most important attachment dimension for predicting

the severity of symptoms is one’s model-of-self. In addition, these

results provide support for Bartholomew’s (1990) conceptualization of the

two dimensional nature of attachment and the implication of Bowlby’s theory

that the intersection of the underlying models of self and other is the basis

for the four basic attachment styles.

Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER - JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207

TRAVMA IN POSAMEZNIK – OTROŠKA SPOLNA ZLORABA ZNOTRAJ DRUŽINE IMA TEŽJE POSLEDICE

Although not all researchers agree about which characteristics of CSA are

most likely to be associated with a poorer prognosis, one trend in the research

suggests that abuse by fathers/stepfathers may lead to greater long-term

effects (Beitchman et al., 1992). In the present study, when the degree of

relatedness between victim and perpetrator is examined, it is apparent that the

relationship between sexual abuse and adjustment is a function of the

influence of intrafamilial sexual abuse; women who were abused within the

family reported significantly more difficulties than women who were abused

by someone outside the family. Women abused by a family member were

especially likely to report problems in the following areas: depression, posttraumatic

stress (i.e., intrusive thoughts and defensive use of avoidance),

anxiety, and an inadequate sense of self and personal identity.

Adult Attachment - A Mediator Between Child Sexual Abuse and Later Psychological Adjustment - DIANE N. ROCHE, MARSHA G. RUNTZ., MICHAEL A. HUNTER - JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 14 No. 2, February 1999 184-207

VRSTE TRAVME IN TRAVMA

Although there is no universally accepted definition of trauma, it is generally understood as a state of being negatively overwhelmed both physically and psychologically: it is the experience of terror, loss of control and utter helplessness during a stressful event that threatens one’s physical and/or psychological integrity. PTSD symptoms include re-experiencing the traumatic event in the form of flashbacks, nightmares, intense bodily or emotional sensations, obsessional preoccupations and behavioural re-enactments. Trauma victims inadvertently tend to re-enact the trauma compulsively by either acting self-destructively, harming others or becoming revictimised. Re-experiencing traumatic events causes persons to alternate between persistent forms of emotional numbing and hyperarousal. Irritability, angry outbursts, restlessness, difficulty concentrating and difficulty sleeping are also common signs. In their attempts to ward off hyperarousal, sexual abuse victims experience withdrawal and detachment from emotions. Such post-traumatic stress symptoms can fragment one’s sense of self and agency and one’s ability to relate to others constructively.

J. Beste; RECOVERY FROM SEXUAL VIOLENCE AND SOCIALLY MEDIATED DIMENSIONS OF GOD’S GRACE: IMPLICATIONS FOR CHRISTIAN COMMUNITIES; [SCE 18.2 (2005) 89–112]

Neither clinicians nor researchers have had an effective system for assessing

the extent of traumatic experience. Alarcon (1997) proposed a typology of PTSD

consisting of six clinical types: depressive, dissociative, somatomorphic, psychotomorphic,

organomorphic, and “neurotic-like.” In addition, he suggests that

substance abuse and personality disorders need to be considered. This classification

appears to be problematic due to extensive overlap; many clients could easily

fit into several categories.

(Eldra P. Solomon, Kathleen M. Heide. Type III Trauma: Toward a More Effective Conceptualization of Psychological Trauma, International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 43(2), 1999 202-210.)

Terr (1991) proposed that there are two basic types of trauma, which she called

Type I and Type II. Type I trauma results from a single event, such as a rape or witnessing

a murder. Survivors of Type I trauma who are 3 years old or older at the

time of the event generally retain complete memory of their experience. These

individuals struggle to make sense out of what happened. They may experience

perceptual errors such as visual hallucinations or time distortions.

According to Terr (1991), Type II trauma results from “repeated exposure to

extreme external events” (p. 15). Survivors of Type II trauma generally have at

least some memory of their experience. Children who sustain Type II trauma use

massive denial, repression, dissociation, identification with the perpetrator, and

aggression against themselves as coping mechanisms. These children are often

diagnosed as having Conduct Disorder, Attention Deficit Disorder, depression, or

a dissociative disorder.

We suggest this distinction because, based on our clinical

experience and those of our colleagues, individuals who survive Type III trauma

suffer more severe psychological effects requiring different treatment strategies.

Examples of Type II trauma include such experiences as repeated fondling by a

neighbor or uncle, or growing up with parents who engage in moderate psychological

or physical abuse.

Type III trauma is more extreme. It results from multiple and pervasive violent

events beginning at an early age and continuing for years. Typically, the childwas

the victim of multiple perpetrators, and one or more are close relatives. The abusive

events were likely frequent, yet unpredictable. Generally, force is used and

the abuse has a sadistic quality. The child may have been threatened with torture or

death, or death of a loved one. Both sexual and physical abuse may have been perpetrated.

Examples would include enduring sadistic ritual abuse by an organized

group or repeated violent physical and sexual abuse by caretakers.

(Eldra P. Solomon, Kathleen M. Heide. Type III Trauma: Toward a More Effective Conceptualization of Psychological Trauma, International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 43(2), 1999 202-210.)

Very few studies have examined the interaction

between emotional abuse or neglect and

sexual abuse on PTSD symptomatology, although

studies suggest that these types of child

maltreatment frequently co-occur (Higgins &

McCabe, 2000).

Definicija travme in PTSD

Although there is no universally accepted definition of trauma, it is generally understood as a state of being negatively overwhelmed both physically and psychologically: it is the experience of terror, loss of control and utter helplessness during a stressful event that threatens one’s physical and/or psychological integrity. PTSD symptoms include re-experiencing the traumatic event in the form of flashbacks, nightmares, intense bodily or emotional sensations, obsessional preoccupations and behavioural re-enactments. Trauma victims inadvertently tend to re-enact the trauma compulsively by either acting self-destructively, harming others or becoming revictimised. Re-experiencing traumatic events causes persons to alternate between persistent forms of emotional numbing and hyperarousal. Irritability, angry outbursts, restlessness, difficulty concentrating and difficulty sleeping are also common signs. In their attempts to ward off hyperarousal, sexual abuse victims experience withdrawal and detachment from emotions. Such post-traumatic stress symptoms can fragment one’s sense of self and agency and one’s ability to relate to others constructively.

Beste, J. (2005). Recovery from sexual violence and socially mediated dimensions of God's grace: Implications for Christian communities. Studies in Christian ethics, 18, 89-112.

VRSTE TRAVME – TRAVME V NAVEZANOSTI

VRSTE TRAVME – TRAVME OB IZGUBI (SMRTI)

Theoretically, a number of

authors have focused on loss as a defining feature of trauma. For example, Lifton

(1988) referred to the “death imprint” (the “radical intrusion of an image or

feeling of threat, or end, to life,” p. 18) as an important aspect of the (trauma) survivor

syndrome. This imprint or intrusion makes it impossible for the survivor to

deny the reality of death and brings him or her face-to-face with feelings of personal

vulnerability and consequent anxiety.

In the empirical literature on grief and bereavement, certain types or modes

of death have been associated, at least in some studies, with bereavement that is

prolonged or has more pathologic outcomes. These include (a) death that is

unexpected, sudden, or untimely; (b) death that is horrific (grotesque) or painful;

(c) death that is violent or stigmatized in some way (e.g., homicide, suicide, or

AIDS); (d) death involving multiple losses; and (e) death of a child (e.g.,

Sanders, 1988, 1993).

In a community study of individuals who experienced conjugal

bereavement, Schut, DeKeijser,Van Den Bout, and Dijkhuis (1991) specifically

examined PTSD symptoms (via self-report) and estimated the proportion of participants

who had “probable” PTSD. They found that 20% to 31% of participants

could be labeled as having probable PTSD during at least one follow-up

point between 4 and 25 months following the death, although most spouses in

this study had died of natural causes.

Kaltman and Bonanno (2000) addressed the dimensions of traumatic loss

that might be associated with elevated depression and PTSD symptoms in

spousal survivors.

Suicide of a family member or friend, more specifically, has been associated

with elevated symptoms. Farberow, Gallagher-Thompson, Gilewski, and

Thompson (1992) followed surviving spouses of individuals who committed

suicide for several years and compared them to survivors of spouses who died of

natural causes and to nonbereaved controls.

To summarize, there is weak support for the hypothesis that unanticipated

death alone meets a “traumatic” stressor criterion (i.e., sufficient to be associated

with PTSD; Kaltman&Bonanno, 2000; Schut et al., 1991). There is stronger

support, however, for the conclusion that violent death is associated with

both PTSD symptoms and enduring distress, especially regarding grief and

depression (Farberow et al., 1992; Kaltman & Bonanno, 2000; Zisook et al.,

1998).

Data gathered in a number of studies indicate that people with one traumatic

event in their histories are likely to have experienced multiple prior and/or subsequent

events (e.g., Green et al., 2000). Furthermore, studies have shown that

multiple exposure to traumatic events, either within the same type of event or

across event types, is associated with higher levels of symptoms than single

exposure (e.g., Follette, Polusny, Bechtle, & Naugle, 1996; McCauley et al.,

1997; Miranda, Green,&Krupnick, 1997).

Njihova raziskava: In conclusion, traumatic loss, defined as loss of a close friend or

familymember by suicide, homicide, or accident, was associated with high levels of intrusion

and reexperiencing symptoms and high rates of acute stress disorder. These

differences were evident in the absence of other life traumas that could account

for these symptoms, in a relatively low-risk, high-resource group. Traumatic

loss was also associated with high levels of subjective distress relative to other

life events. Violent loss seems to precipitate traditional traumatic

stress-response symptoms and to be appropriately classified as a traumatic

stressor. Symptoms may continue for prolonged periods and could potentially

be reduced by psychological interventions.

VRSTE TRAVME – ZLORABA IN NADLEGOVANJE

Sexual assault is any sexual act forced on a person against his or her will (Bowker, 1983).

Child sexual abuse is a heterogeneous label,

including single incident stranger assaults, in

addition to cases of intrafamilial abuse lasting

for years (Kendall-Tackett, Williams, &

Finkelhor, 1993; Rowan & Foy, 1993). Sexual

abuse may include a wide range of Criterion-A

traumatic events including fondling, coercive

sexual contact, and penetration (Kendall-

Tackett et al., 1993; Rowan & Foy, 1993).

Sexual harassment has become a major social, legal, and mental health problem because of its high

prevalence and its negative consequences for victims. These consequences can include decreased

productivity, loss of job, decreased income, and impaired psychological and physical well-being. Despite

evidence from empirical studies that victims often exhibit posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)

symptoms, some have argued that sexual harassment does not constitute legitimate trauma.We argue

that many forms of sexual harassment meet the diagnostic Criteria A1 and A2 of PTSD. Finally, the

DSM-IV trauma criterion is explicated, and its relationship with sexual harassment and its effects are

discussed.

Sexual harassment has become an increasingly important

issue over the past two decades. Over 10,000 people

made complaints of sexual harassment in 1992, and

complaints bywomen have nearly more than doubled from

5,603 in 1989 to 14,420 in 1994 (Andrew&Andrew, 1997;

Simon, 1996). Sexual harassment occurs in many different

settings: 51% of family practice female resident physicians,

64% of females in the U.S. military, 70% of female

officeworkers, and88%of female nurses report having experienced

sexual harassment (Dan, Pinsof,&Riggs, 1995;

Piotrkowski, 1998; Pryor, 1995; Vukovich, 1996).

Definicije spolnega nadlegovanja:

Legal and Regulatory Definitions

The law proscribing sexual harassment derives from

Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964.Title VII prohibits

discrimination “with respect to : : : terms, conditions, or

privileges of employment : : :” because of an individual’s

sex, race, religion, and so forth. (Title VII, Civil Rights Act

x2000-2(a). Although sexual harassment is not explicitly

mentioned in the Act, courts later interpreted sexual harassment

to be subsumed because it is gender-related.

According to the U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity

Commission (1980), sexual harassment is defined as

Unwelcome sexual advances, requests for sexual favors,

and other verbal or physical conduct of a sexual nature;

when cooperation or submission was an implicit or explicit

condition of employment; was used as a basis for

the employment-related decisions; or when the conduct

has the purpose or effect of unreasonably interfering with

a person’s work performance or creating an intimidating,

hostile or offensive working environment. (p. 74676)

Psychological Definitions of Sexual Harassment

A psychological definition does not focus on the incident

itself but rather, attends to the victim’s evaluation

of the situation such that the victim’s evaluation is influenced

by factors like ambiguity, perceived threat, and loss

(Fitzgerald, Swan, & Fischer, 1995). Fitzgerald, Swan,

et al. (1997) defined sexual harassment psychologically as

“unwanted sex-related behavior at work that is appraised

by the recipient as offensive, exceeding her resources, or

threatening her well-being” (p. 15).

DSM-IV Criteria for Posttraumatic Stress

Disorder (PTSD)

Harassment victims have been described as suffering

from a “posttrauma syndrome” (Hamilton, Alagna,

King, & Lloyd, 1987). A PTSD model of the sequelae of

sexual harassment has been used to attempt to account

for effects such as flashbacks, sleep disturbances, and

emotional numbing (Gutek & Koss, 1993; Koss, 1990).

Clinical researchers have reported that sexual harassment

victims are frequently meeting the symptom criteria for

PTSD (Dansky & Kilpatrick, 1997). The Diagnostic and

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American

Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for PTSD are

(A) The person has been exposed to a traumatic

event in which both of the following were

present:

(1) the person experienced, witnessed, or was

confronted with an event or events that involved

actual or threatened death or serious

injury, or a threat to the physical integrity of

self or others;

(2) the person’s response involved intense fear,

helplessness, or horror (pp. 427–428);

(B) Reexperiencing the event and severe distress;

(C) Avoidance of associated stimuli; and

(D) Hyperarousal.

VRSTE TRAVME – ZANEMARJANJE

Definition of Neglect

There is little agreement on the definition and

measurement of neglect (Costin, Karger, & Stoesz,

1996; National Research Council, 1993). One of the

most important points of disagreement concerns

whether neglect should be defined and measured in a

way that includes injury or harm to a child as compared

to definition and measurement solely on the

basis of the behavior of the caregiver (Straus &

Kaufman Kantor, 2005). Another unresolved issue is

whether the neglectful behavior must be intentional.

VRSTE TRAVME – KOMPLEKSNA TRAVMA

DEFINICIJA KOMPLEKSNE TRAVME IN SIMPTOMOV

Complex trauma refers to a type of trauma that occurs repeatedly and cumulatively, usually over a period of time and within specific relationships and contexts. The term came into being over the past decade as researchers found that some forms of trauma were much more pervasive and complicated than others (Herman,1992a, 1992b).

The diagnostic conceptualization of CPTSD/DESNOS as defined for the field trial consisted of seven different problem areas shown by research to be associated with early interpersonal trauma (Herman, 1992a, 1992b):

1. alterations in the regulation of affective impulses, including difficulty with modulation of anger and self-destructiveness. This category has come to include all methods used for emotional regulation and self-soothing, including addictions and self-harming behaviors that are, paradoxically, often life saving;

2. alterations in attention and consciousness leading to amnesias and dissociative episodes and depersonalization. This category includes emphasis on dissociative responses different than those found in the DSM criteria for PTSD. Its inclusion in the CPTSD conceptualization incorporates the findings regarding dissociation that were mentioned earlier, namely, that dissociation tends to be related to prolonged and severe interpersonal abuse occurring during childhood and, secondarily, that children are more prone to dissociation than are adults;

3. alterations in self perception, such as a chronic sense of guilt and responsibility, and ongoing feelings of intense shame. Chronically abused individuals often incorporate the lessons of abuse into their sense of self and self-worth (Courtois, 1979a, 1979b; Pearlman, 2001);

4. alterations in perception of the perpetrator, including incorporation of his or her belief system. This criterion addresses the complex relationships and belief systems that ensue following repetitive and premeditated abuse at the hands of primary caretakers;

5. alterations in relationship to others, such as not being able to trust and not being able to feel intimate with others. Another “lesson of abuse” internalized by victim/survivors is that people are venal and self-serving, out to get what they can by whatever means including using/abusing others;

6. somatization and/or medical problems. These somatic reactions and medical conditions may relate directly to the type of abuse suffered and any physical damage that was caused or they may be more diffuse. They have been found to involve all major body systems;

7. alterations in systems of meaning. Chronically abused individuals often feel hopeless about finding anyone to understand them or their suffering. They despair of ever being able to recover from their psychic anguish.

OTROK in TRAVMA, MLADOSTNIK in TRAVMA, ODRASEL in TRAVMA

Child hood trau mas include phys i cal mal treat ment (Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor, 1995), sex ual assault or moles ta -tion (Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor, 1995; Neumann, Houskamp, Pollock, & Briere, 1996), life-threat ening acci dents (Winje & Ulvik, 1998), the unex pected death of close friends or fam ily mem bers (Appelbaum & Burns, 1991), life-threat ening ill ness (Stuber, Nader, Houskamp, & Pynoos, 1996), disas ter (Green et al.,1994; LaGreca, Silverman, Vernberg, & Prinstein, 1996), domes tic vio lence (Famularo, Fenton, Kinscherff, Ayoub, & Barnum 1994), and com mu nity vio lence (Cooley, Turner, & Beidel, 1995; Nader, Pynoos, Fair banks, & Fred er ick, 1990).

Child Mal treatment, Other Trauma Ex po sure, and Posttraumatic Symptomatology Among Children With Oppositional De fi ant

and At tention Def i cit Hy peractivity Dis or ders

Julian D. Ford (University of Con necticut School of Med icine), Robert Racusin, Cynthia G. Ellis, Wil liam B. Daviss, Jessica Reiser, Amy Fleischer (Dartmouth Med i cal School), Julie Thomas (Youngs town State Uni ver sity).

TRAUMA DURING CHILDHOOD

To date, much of the research in the area of traumatic stress has

focused specifically on events that arise during childhood. Although

trauma can arise at any point in the life cycle (Janoff-Bulman, 1992),

many investigators believe that exposure to trauma during childhood

may be especially harmful. The importance of events that occur in

childhood is suggested both by the work of Brown and Harris (1978)

on early parental loss and Bowlby’s (1980) research on attachment

theory.

According to Brown and Harris (1978), early childhood is an

exceptionally salient developmental period. In particular, they argued

that during early life, a child’s parents are often the primary source for

learning effective ways of exercising personal control. This is important

because a strong sense of control helps people to anticipate potentially

stressful experiences or conditions, take preventive steps to

avoid them, and confront and deal with the adversity that does occur

(Ross, Mirowsky, & Goldsteen, 1990). Consequently, the lessons

learned during this early period of interaction between parents and

children may have lasting effects on one’s ability to handle adversity.

Bowlby (1980) also considered childhood a particularly important

and vulnerable developmental stage. According to him, childhood is

an important period for determining the nature of one’s social relationships

later in life. Specifically, he maintained that a child’s relationship

with his or her parents serves as a prototype for the development

of social ties in adult years. Parent-child relationships that are

intimate and caring tend to foster a sense of trust and security that

facilitates the development of interpersonal closeness throughout the

life course (for a recent reviewof this perspective, also see Reis&Patrick,

1996). Therefore, children may be particularly vulnerable to

traumatic events, especially those of an interpersonal nature such as

exposure to physical violence from one’s parents because they could

have lasting effects on one’s ability to form and maintain meaningful

and supportive social relationships throughout life.

In studying the consequences of childhood trauma in general and

childhood physical violence specifically,much of the current research

has focused on immediate or short-term effects of exposure to violence

on childhood behavior and psychosocial adjustment (e.g.,

Conaway & Hansen, 1989; Lamphear, 1985). Recently, however,

investigators have become increasingly interested in the potential

long-term consequences of childhood exposure to traumatic events.

Profound loss covers a broad spectrum of childhood experiences.

It can come from separation from parents or family through death,

divorce, foster care placement, or the adverse political and economic

circumstances that are associated with emigration.Recent data suggest

that childhood loss of a parent from natural causes is as strongly

associated with PTSD symptoms as children’s reactions to natural

disasters (3). Profound loss can occur as an outcome of parental mental

illness, sometimes quietly and without acknowledgment as when a

parent is lost to depression, or with violent upheaval, as is often the

case with alcohol or other substance use disorders. Today, abandonment

and neglect due to these addictions are the most common reasons for

foster care placement (4). Loss can include physical injury to the self, in

which a part of the body is, or is believed to be, damaged or altered. The

initial loss frequently sets off a series of events (e.g., hospitalization,

relocation, foster care placement) which in turn lead to further loss.

TRAVMA IN SPOMIN

TRAVMA IN KONCENTRACIJA

ZNAČILNOSTI STORILCA – SPOLNA ZLORABA

Osebe, ki spolno zlorabljajo in navezanost

In support of the contention that sexual offenders

had poor parent-child attachments,

there is a considerable body of literature indicating

a variety of disruptive experiences in the

childhood of these offenders. Langevin et al.

(1984), for instance, found that rapists had quite

inadequate parents with whom they failed to

identify. Various other researchers (Awad,

Saunders, & Levene, 1984; Bass & Levant, 1992;

Finkelhor, 1984; Knight, Prentky, Schnieder, &

Rosenberg, 1983; Lang & Langevin, 1991;

Protter & Travin, 1987; Saunders, Awad, &

White, 1986; Tingle, Barnard, Robbins,

Newman, & Hutchinson, 1986) have reported

disruptive and abusive family environments in

the childhoods of sexual offenders. Drunkenness,

physical and sexual abuse, inconsistency,

emotional neglect or rejection, hostility, criminal

activities, social isolation, and various other

problematic circumstances were found to characterize

the family backgrounds of sexual offenders.

More specifically, Marshall, Serran,

and Cortoni (2000) found poorer attachments to

fathers than to mothers among child molesters,

and Smallbone and Dadds (in press) showed

that insecure attachments to fathers led to the

enactment of coercive sexual behavior in adulthood.

In a more recent study, Smallbone and

Dadds (2000) again demonstrated that insecure

childhood attachments were significantly associated

with coercive sexual behavior. These results

remained true even after the influences of

antisociality and aggression were partialed out.

Clearly, sexual offenders have experienced a

significant incidence of problematic relations

with their parents during their childhood, and

these experiences appear to be causally significant

in the development of their offensive

behaviors.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

Zlorabljeni zlorabljajo?

It has certainly been demonstrated that sexual

offenders report far higher rates of personal

experience with being sexually victimized as

children than do other males (Hanson & Slater,

1988). However, much of these data result from

rather simple questions put to the offenders

whomay have a vested interest in exaggerating

childhood sexual abuse; for example, they see

this as diminishing their responsibility for their

ownoffending.

Bentovim and Williams (1998) found that the

majority of children who were sexually abused

came from homes where they had been abused

or neglected.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

Although the majority of children who are

subject to sexual abuse suffer both immediate

and long-term consequences (Beitchman et al.,

1992; Conte, 1988), these effects are not always

seen by the victim as resulting from the abuse.

Indeed, we might expect vulnerable children

who lack a sense of self-worth to blame themselves

for these effects and, as a consequence, to

judge the abuse to not have harmed them.Many

child molesters who were sexually abused as

children claim that it did them no harm, and so,

they see their own abusive behavior as not

harmful to the victim.

If sexual offenders have greater experience

with being sexually molested as children, and if

this satisfies their need for attention, they may

construe the abuse as positive at least in some

respects. Their low self-esteem may cause them

to attribute any unfortunate consequences not

to the abuse or the abuser but rather to some defect

in their own character. Both their need for

attention and their low self-esteem may allow

these children to perceive the abuse in positive

terms. Insofar as they perceive the abuse in this

way, it may encourage them to see sex between

an adult and a child as nonharmful and even

beneficial, thereby removing one significant

constraint against sexual offending later in their

life. In addition, if they did derive pleasure from

their own abuse, subsequently during masturbation

they may fantasize about sexual contact

between an adult and a child. In these fantasies,

they may initially portray themselves in the role

of the victim and then later see themselves as the

offender.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

Consistent with this line of reasoning,

Smallbone and McCabe (2000) found that those

sexual offenders whowere themselves sexually

abused as children reported a significantly earlier

onset of masturbation than did offenders

who were not sexually abused. The results of

this study by Smallbone and McCabe are consistent

with some important links in our theory.

Their data revealed that insecure parent-child

attachments among sexual offenders led to a

marked increase in the likelihood that these

boys would be sexually abused, which in turn

resulted in an early onset of masturbation

among these boys. In addition, because

Smallbone and Dadds (2000, in press) had

shown that insecure attachments in childhood

lead to adult sexual offending, it is reasonable to

suggest that there is a pathway involving insecure

attachments ® a greater risk to be sexually

abused ® heightened sexualization (most particularly

masturbation) ® which finally results

in adult sexual offending. The next two sections

attempt to fill in the gap between early masturbation

(i.e., sexualization) and adult sexual

offending.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

Deviantna spolna aktivnost

Abel and Rouleau (1990) found that

between 40% and 50% of child molesters and

30% of rapists reported an interest in sexually

deviant activities before the age of 18 years, and

other reports are consistent with this early origin

of deviant sexuality.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

SEX AS A COPING STRATEGY (Spolnost- kot strategija »soočanja« s težavami)

Children who are deprived of love and affection,

as most sexual offenders seem to have

been, are quite likely to turn to self-stimulation

as a way of making themselves feel better.

Children learn quite early to masturbate and

that this behavior is pleasurable (Goldman &

Goldman, 1982, 1988; Masters, Johnson, &

Kolodny, 1985). Anything that is pleasurable

can readily serve as a distraction from problems.

If deprived children find solace in masturbation,

they will likely soon recognize that this

is a way to avoid facing other problems; it will

serve as an escape, albeit temporarily, from difficult

issues. Because masturbation is a highly

reliable source of pleasure guaranteed to divert

attention away from difficulties, the criteria for

defining an issue as a problem and using masturbation

to relieve it can be expected to progressively

expand to all manner of problems

and to less and less intense problems.

As we have seen, sexual offenders display an earlier

onset of masturbation and a higher frequency

during adolescence, and we consider this to be a

result of them having learned to use sex as away

of coping initially with their deprived experiences

and later with awhole range of issues. Sex

can, therefore, be expected to be used as a primary

coping strategy by sexual offenders.

If we are correct that vulnerable young males

who are to become adult sexual offenders find,

in masturbating, the comfort and relief they

cannot otherwise obtain, then escape from dis-tress by masturbating should be negatively reinforced.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

Negativno, pozitivno pogojevanje in spolnost

Negative reinforcement, to remind

readers, occurs when a behavior leads to the

cessation of a noxious state (Skinner, 1969).

When a person has a panic attack on an elevator

and flees from the elevator at the next floor, the

consequent reduction in panic reinforces avoiding

elevators, and an elevator phobia may become

entrenched. Each time a child or teenaged

boy uses masturbation to avoid having to confront

a difficult situation or to escape temporarily

from distress, he is inadvertently engaging

in a procedure that will negatively reinforce

masturbation as a response to any and all upsetting

events. As a result, masturbation, or for that

matter any sexual activity, will become an established

coping response. As we have seen, recent

evidence suggests that sexual offenders do,

indeed, use sex as a coping strategy.

However, it is not just that masturbating negatively

reinforces the use of sex as a coping strategy.

Masturbation clearly induces a pleasurable

state, and anything (e.g., the content of fantasies)

that is consistently associated with this

state is likely to acquire a positive valence. That

is, masturbation also functions as a positively

reinforcing experience just as the early conditioning

theorists claimed.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

Negativno, pozitivno pogojevanje, spolnost in vedenje

Although the early

conditioning theorists (Abel & Blanchard, 1974;

McGuire et al., 1965) may have been wrong

about the precise outcome of their theory (i.e.,

the entrenchment of a highly specific sexual

preference), they were probably correct in suggesting

that the content of masturbatory fantasies

had some guiding influence on behavior.

These early theorists, however, focused exclusively

on the particular sexual elements of the

fantasies because they appear to have believed

that sexually deviant behaviors were motivated

exclusively by sexual desire. Laws and Marshall

(1990), on the other hand, describe a far more

complex conditioning theory that allows a

broader inclusion of stimuli (e.g., the exercise of

power and control, the humiliation of the victim,

the expression of aggression) into the content

of masturbatory fantasies. Any repeated

content (whether sexual or otherwise) of

masturbatory fantasies is likely to become entrenched

as a result of both the negatively reinforcing

effects of escape from distress and the

positively reinforcing properties of the pleasurable

experience of sexual arousal.

Very few, if any, human behaviors are motivated

by a single desire; almost all behaviors have

multiple motives. Sexual activities in particular

seem to serve many purposes (Neubeck, 1974).

Amongother things, sexmaybe sought to achieve

feelings of intimacy or to obtain affection, to alleviate

boredom or a sense of frustration (nonsexual),

as a way to obtain self-affirmation, to

achieve a sense of conquest, or as we have suggested,

to escape from problems.

Deviant sex may also be driven by or associated

with a need to exercise power and control

over another person, a chance to explore “forbidden”

acts (e.g., anal sex occurs at an unusually

high rate in sexual abuse), and as a chance to

vent anger or to humiliate someone. Gratuitous

physical abuse appears to be common in sexual

assaults (Christie, Marshall, & Lanthier, 1979;

Marshall & Christie, 1981), and rapists typically

indicate that their primary motive in sexually

assaulting a female is to degrade and humiliate

her as a symbol of either all women or a particular

woman who has offended them (Darke,

1990; Marshall & Darke, 1982). Groth (1979;

Groth&Burgess, 1977a) has been an advocate of

the view that rape is a pseudo sexual behavior.

He believes it is the expression of power and the

exercise of control over women, rather than the

satisfaction of sexual desires, that drives a rapist.

To support this account, Groth and Burgess

(1977b) point to the fact that sexual dysfunctions

occur in the offender quite frequently during

rapes, and many of our clients have told us that

they are rarely satisfied by the sexual release

they obtain from raping a woman.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

Doživljanje, čustvovanje

Child molesters, on the other hand, appear to

experience strong sexual satisfaction from molesting

children. This does not mean, however,

that other aspects of the abuse are irrelevant.

Howells (1979), for example, found that child

molesters feel emotionally congruent with children

but afraid of potential adult partners and

threatened by sex with an adult. Araji and

Finklehor (1985) propose in their more general

theory that emotional congruence with children

is essential for child molesters to offend. One

important consequence of this is that child molesters

feel in control when they have sex with

children, a feeling they do not experience in sex

with adults, or for that matter, in most other aspects

of their life. This feeling of control is emphasized

by the fact that the molester has the

power to direct the child to engage in whatever

activities he wishes. This power derives both

from the fact that he is an adult and children are

trained to follow the orders of adults and from

the likelihood that the child will feel threatened

by amanwhois sexually assaulting him or her.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

We suggest that the higher frequency of

masturbatory activities during adolescence increases

the likelihood that sexual offenders will

incorporate other elements (e.g., forbidden sexual

acts, power and control, the expression of

aggression) into their sexual fantasies. This may

be done to avoid boredom with the same content

of frequently repeated fantasies, but it is

also likely to occur given their experience as a

rejected or neglected child. Such children, as we

have seen, lack a sense of self-worth and are deficient

in interpersonal skills. They are unlikely,

therefore, to find satisfaction in peer-aged appropriate

relationships and will turn more and

more to fantasy to fulfill their needs. Some of

these children, as a result of their feelings of inadequacy,

may feel capable of relating to weaker

people, and they will thus feel emotionally congruent

only with children. Therefore, the sexual

fantasies of these adolescents may incorporate

children over whom they exercise control and

whom they portray in their fantasies as willing

and eager to have sex with adults. Other vulnerable

adolescents may feel angry about the way

their parents have treated them, and they may

blame women for their lack of intimate affection

and sexuality. We (Garlick, Marshall, &

Thornton, 1996) have, indeed, found that some

sexual offenders blame women for the loneliness

they experience. These adolescents will

likely incorporate the expression of their anger

into their sexual fantasies. This may take the

form of excessive control over the sexual partner

in their fantasies and may, over time, becomemore

cruel and include elements of humiliation

and degradation. It is easy to see how

such a process and an associated severe lack of

self-confidence might make such a male begin

to interpret various behaviors by women as rejection,

leading to the conclusion that all women

are contemptuous of him.

Rapists certainly hold negative and hostile

views of women (Burt, 1980; Marshall &

Hambley, 1996), and child molesters, aswehave

seen, feel emotionally comfortable only with

children (Howells, 1979).

Wright and Schneider claim that sexual

offenders progressively incorporate elements

into their sexual fantasies that serve to bolster

their self-esteem and justify their sexually offensive

behaviors or desires. In an examination of

this account, Wright and Schneider found that

the fantasies of sexual offenders did, indeed,

contain portrayals of victims as compliant,

strongly sexually motivated, and as sexually

provocative. Although Wright and Schneider

did not specifically examine this, it could be that

sexual offenders also include elements of violence,

degradation, and the enactment of forbidden

sexual acts into their fantasies.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

FROM A DISPOSITION TO ENACTMENT

Two sets of factors seem to be important: disinhibiting

influences and an opportunity to

offend.

Disinhibiting Factors

Araji and Finkelhor (1985) propose that for a

man to commit a sexual offense, he must overcome

whatever inhibitions he has against these

behaviors. Similarly, Barbaree (1990) suggests

that sexual arousal to deviant acts occurs only in

those men whose constraints against sexual

arousal are disinhibited. Intoxication is the

disinhibitor most familiar to the ordinary citizen.

Alcohol ingestion disinhibits social constraints

(Firestone, Keyes, & Korneluk, 1999),

encourages the expression of aggression

(Bushman & Cooper, 1990), and facilitates sexual

arousal (Wilson, 1981).

We (Barbaree, Marshall,

Yates, & Lightfoot, 1983) demonstrated

that alcohol intoxication increased sexual

arousal to rape cues in nondeviant males, and it

has been found that as many as 50% or more of

the sexual offenders were intoxicated at the time

of their offense (Amir 1967; Christie et al., 1979;

Johnson, Gibson, & Linden, 1978).

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

Kognitivna distorzija in zloraba

According to

Wright and Schneider’s analysis, sexual fantasies

are the primary source of the cognitive

distortions (see Ward, Hudson, Johnston, &

Marshall, 1997, for a description of these distortions)

that clinicians and researchers report so

commonly in sexual offenders and that some

see as serving to justify and maintain sexual offending.

These cognitive distortions, then, feed

back into the processes that lead to sexual offending

and are elaborated further during

masturbatory fantasies. It is the conditioned enhancement

of these elements of the fantasies of

sexual offenders, rather than the strictly sexual

elements, thatwebelieved entrench the disposition

to offend.

Abel and his colleagues (Abel,

Becker, & Cunningham-Rathner, 1984; Abel

et al., 1989) have described the typical cognitive

distortions of child molesters. These involve a

belief that children are sexually disposed, behave

sexually provocatively toward adult

males, initiate sexual acts with adults, enjoy

such activities, and are not harmed by sex with

an adult.

Abel sees these distortions as serving

to justify sexual abuse and as allowing the offender

to continue his abusive behavior free of

guilt and remorse. More specifically, Hartley

(1998) describes how the cognitive distortions

of incest offenders serve to overcome internal

inhibitions about molesting. In other words,

these distortions are thought to function as

disinhibitors of child molesting tendencies.

Similarly, rapists have been shown to hold

views of women and their sexuality that can be

expected to facilitate rape (Bumby, 1996; Burt,

1984; Dewhurst, Moore, & Alfano, 1992; Marshall

& Hambly, 1996; Scott & Tetreault, 1987).

Attitudes condoning violence toward women,

feelings of hostility toward women, support for

rape myths, and distorted perceptions of

women have all been found to be common

among rapists. Again, these distortions can be

understood as facilitating sexual abuse and justifying

continued offending. As such, the distortions

of rapists serve to disinhibit constraints

against rape. Sundberg, Barbaree, and Marshall

(1991) demonstrated that blaming the victim in

a rape led men to show significant increases in

sexual arousal to rape depictions, thereby demonstrating

that victim blame served to disinhibit

responses to rape.

Anger has been found to precede rapes

(Pithers, Beal, Armstrong, & Petty, 1989), and

we (Yates, Barbaree, & Marshall, 1984) showed

that when angered by females, nondeviant

males subsequently displayed very strong

arousal to rape scenes. When not angered, these

same nondeviant males showed little arousal to

rape. Thus, anger toward females served to

disinhibit sexual arousal to rape. Similarly,

Proulx and his colleagues (McKibbon, Proulx,&

Lusignan, 1994; Proulx, McKibbon,&Lusignan,

1996) have shown that various mood states trigger

deviant sexual fantasies in sexual offenders

whose fantasies are otherwise absent or normal.

Looman (1999) essentially replicated Proulx’s

findings.

Priložnost in zloraba, zlorabljanje

However, it is important to note that it is

the probability of deviant, rather than normative

fantasies and acts that increase in sexual offenders

when they are experiencing stress or

negative emotions. Once in a disinhibited state,

a sexual offender will only offend when he has

the opportunity to.

Negative mood states appear to trigger deviant

fantasies in sexual offenders, which in turn

lead them to seize or seek to create an opportunity

to offend. Pithers et al. (1989) showed that

deviant fantasizing often preceded sexual offending,

and Abel and Rouleau (1990) reported

that sexual offenders typically developed their

deviant sexual fantasies prior to offending.

The confluence of the variety of factors we

have outlined (i.e., a history of childhood neglect

or abuse, the consequent lowered sense of

self-worth, an incapacity to meet various needs

in prosocial ways, along with the tendency to

turn to sexwhenin distress, a conditioned desire

for deviant sex, and the presence of a disinhibited

state) will lead amale to seek or take advantage

of an opportunity to sexually offend. It is

clear that some and possibly most sexual offenders

deliberately create opportunities to offend.

Some do this with full awareness; they

plan to offend, manipulate situations to get others

out of the way and to get access to a victim

alone, and groom victims or coerce them into

complying. Others operate at a lower level of

awareness, whereby they either actually develop

an opportunity to offend or simply to allow

events to unfold in a way that produces an

opportunity. Ward, Hudson, and Marshall

(1995) illustrate how this diminished awareness

(called cognitive deconstruction) permits the generation

of an opportunity while allowing the offender

to maintain that he did not deliberately

create the opportunity.

We have also seen numerous

sexual offenders who have stumbled on

an unexpected opportunity to offend. For many

of them, these unplanned opportunities oc

occurred

after their offending had already been

established and they simply seized the chance

to offend. In some but few, fortuitous opportunities

presented themselves as the first chance

to offend.

In whatever way opportunities occur, the

critical point is that however strongly disposed

a man is to rape a woman or molest a child, he

cannot do so unless an opportunity is present.

When an opportunity occurs, the cognitive distortions

developed during fantasizing (i.e., that

children enjoy sex with adults, that all women

secretly desire to be raped) facilitate, along with

other disinhibiting influences, the decision to

seize the change to offend. Once the man has

offended, and particularly if he avoids detection,

it is likely that the experience will feed subsequent

masturbatory fantasies, further entrenching

his disposition to engage in sexually

offensive behaviors. No doubt, somemen are so

shocked, fearful, or distressed over their initial

offense that they do not offend again. This

would be most likely to occur in those men for

whom the disposition to offend is weakest. For

those whose disposition is strong,wecan expect

that subsequent to the initial offense, conditioning

processes, occurring as a result of masturbating

to fantasies of the offense, will further entrench

the tendency to sexually offend.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

[pic]

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

[pic]

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

POSLEDICE TRAVME

The psychological sequelae of CSA often persist into adulthood and include general psychological distress, anxiety, depression, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and somatization. Difficulties in social and interpersonal functioning, fear and distrust of others, low self-esteem, and feelings of hostility are also common in CSA survivors (for review, see Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Polusny & Follette, 1995). Reactions following sexual assault in adulthood are similar and may manifest in symptoms of depression, fear, anxiety, and PTSD as well as problems with social adjustment and self-esteem (Goodmanet al., 1993; Kilpatrick, Saunders,Veronen, Best,& Von, 1987; Koss, 1993). Women who have been physically assaulted exhibit similar symptomatology including high levels of depression, fear, anxiety, and PTSD-like symptoms including chronic fatigue and tension, intense startle reactions, disturbed sleeping and eating patterns, and nightmares (Goodman et al., 1993; Stark & Flitcraft, 1988). Physical violence also undermines a woman’s sense of trust and has a dramatic impact on her social interactions (Goodman et al., 1993) (The Revictimization of Child Sexual Abuse Survivors: An Examination of the Adjustment of College Women With Child Sexual Abuse, Adult Sexual Assault, and Adult Physical Abuse, CHILD

MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2000)

The available data have revealed that there is a dual effect of adversity including opportunities for growth apart from symptoms (Somerfield & McCrae, 2000). Correlational research has indicated a broad array of changes following negative events, including HIV/AIDS, cancer, marrow transplantation, newborn intensive care, impaired fertility, rape, and child sexual abuse (Affleck, Tennen, & Rowe, 1991; Armeli, Gunthert, & Cohen, 2001; Burt & Katz, 1987; Collins et al., 1990; Curbow, Somerfield, Baker, Wingard, & Legro, 1993; Ebersole & Flores, 1991; Lechman et al., 1993; McMillen, Zuravin, & Rideout, 1995; Mendola, Tennen, Affleck, McCann, & Fitzgerald, 1990; Park, Cohen, & Murch, 1996; Siegel & Schrimshaw, 2000; Thompson, 1985). Longitudinal studies have shown follow-up benefits of positive views following crisis, especially in patient outcomes (Affleck, Allen,Tennen, McGrade,&Ratzan, 1985; Affleck, Tennen, Croog, & Levine, 1987; Bower et al., 1998, 2003; Tennen, Affleck, Urrows, Higgins, & Mendola, 1992). Very fewstudies have examined the positive gains from violent events, in particular those involving killing and massive destruction. A survey found that, mediated by negative religious coping,war trauma contributed to diminished hope inKosovar refugees in the United States (Ai, Peterson,&Huang, 2003).

Recent literature has suggested that many of the psychological effects of

childhood sexual abuse, such as self-destructive behaviors, post-traumatic

stress disorder (PTSD), anxiety, interpersonal difficulties, and sexual dysfunction,

may begin with cognitive distortions about the self and the world

that become part of a child’s cognitive schema (Smucker, Dancu, Foa, &

Niederee, 1995). Further research proposes that these schema distortions

may contribute to the emotional distress experienced by many adult survivors

of sexual abuse (Briere & Elliott, 1994).

For more than two decades, research has consistently found that childhood

sexual abuse (CSA) is associated with poor psychological outcome in adult

populations. This is especially true of research that has investigated the relationship

between mental illness and a history of CSA (e.g., Bryer, Nelson,

Miller,&Krol, 1987; Chu&Dill, 1990; Fry, 1993; Mancini,Van Ameringen,&

MacMillan, 1995; Ogata et al., 1990). Although no abuse-specific syndrome

has been discovered, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptomatology

and sexualized behaviors are consistently shown to have a strong association

with CSA (for literature reviews, see Beitchman et al., 1992; Browne &

Finkelhor, 1986; Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993).

Finkelhor, 1986; Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993). To date,

there has been a dearth of empirical investigation on the long-term psychological

consequences associated with childhood physical abuse (CPA)

(Malinosky-Rummell&Hansen, 1993), although research on the short-term

effects of CPA has been extensive (see Ammerman, Cassisi, Hersen, & Van

Hasselt, 1986). The Relationship Between Adult Psychological Adjustment and Childhood Sexual Abuse, Childhood Physical Abuse, and

Family-of-Origin Characteristics SHANE KAMSNER MARITA P. MCCABE

Research over the past two decades supports the relationship between child

abuse and a variety of long-term intrapersonal difficulties including depression,

anxiety, substance abuse, anger, dissociation, low self-esteem, and suicidality

(Jumper, 1995; Neumann, Houskamp, Pollock, & Briere, 1996).

Quite predictably, women with a history of IPV as compared to women

who were nontraumatized had higher levels of psychopathology, reported

more current relational aggression, and functioned more poorly. Childhood

maltreatment also appears to have a significant impact on women’s current

functioning. CSA was associated with increased anxiety sensitivity, which

has been identified as a risk factor for the development of panic disorder

(Schmidt, 1999). Emotional neglect during childhood was associated with

more dissociative and depressive symptoms in this sample. Similar findings

about emotional neglect have been reported in relation to major depression

(Bernet & Stein, 1999), and psychological maltreatment has been linked to

self-depreciation (Higgins & McCabe, 2000b). This group also showed a

pattern of increasing difficulties with experience of more types of childhood

maltreatment. Although childhood physical and sexual abuse have typically

received more attention in the literature, these findings underscore the importance

of attending to multiple types of childhood maltreatment and to the

cumulative effect of such experiences.

As with adults, traumatised children exhibit a spectrum

of psychological consequences of the trauma, including

altered attentional processes, deficits in cognitive systems

necessary for learning, inefficient memory systems, defi-

cits in affective responsiveness, and so on. However,

there are very few research studies of generic memory in

traumatised children.

Research has demonstrated that exposure to traumatic events during childhood

and adolescence is associated with severe and devastating emotional and

behavioral outcomes. While the majority of trauma victims do not go on to

develop chronic psychopathology, traumatic events such as child sexual abuse,

physical abuse=assault, and witnessing violence increase the risk for the

development of posttraumatic stress and other anxiety symptoms (Kilpatrick,

Ruggiero, et al., 2003; McLeer, Deblinger, Atkins, Foa, & Ralphe, 1988;

McLeer et al., 1998), depression (Kilpatrick, Ruggiero, et al., 2003; Tebbutt,

Swanston, Oates, & O’Toole, 1997; Wozencraft, Wagner, & Pellegrin, 1991),

suicide attempts (Oddone Paolucci, Genuis, & Violato, 2001), substance abuse

(Kilpatrick, Ruggiero, et al., 2003), and delinquent behavior (Kilpatrick,

Saunders, & Smith, 2003).These emotional and behavioral difficulties can be

associated with significant and lasting disruption in children’s normal development,

as well as dysfunction and distress well into adulthood (Beitchman

et al., 1992; Briere & Elliott, 1994; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Polusny &

Follette,1995; Saunders et al.,1999).

Abstract: There are very few re search stud ies that have eval u ated the re -

lationships be tween mul ti ple forms of child hood mal treat -

ment and psy chological ad justment in adult hood. This study

eval u ates the in ter re la tion ships be tween five dif fer ent types of

child mal treatment (sex ual abuse, phys ical abuse, psy cho log

ical mal treat ment, ne glect, wit ness ing fam ily vi o lence) in a

community sam ple of women and men (N = 175). The re la -

tion ships be tween the re ported ex perience of these forms of

mal treat ment in child hood, fam ily char ac ter is tics dur ing

child hood, and cur rent psy cho log i cal ad justment (trauma

symptomatology and self-de preciation) were as sessed. As hy -

poth e sized, fam ily char ac ter is tics pre dicted mal treat ment

scores and ad justment, and mal treat ment scores pre dicted

adjustment af ter con trol ling for fam ily en vi ron ment. There

were high cor re la tions be tween scores on the five mal treat ment

scales. Re sults high light the need to as sess all forms of mal -

treatment when look ing at re lationships of mal treatment to

adjustment and the im por tance of child hood fa mil ial en vi

ronment for the long-term ad justment of adults.

Multitype mal treat ment refers to the coex is tence of

one or more mal treat ment types (Hig gins & McCabe,

1998, in press-b). A review of stud ies in which adult

respon dents reported hav ing expe rienced mul tiple

forms of child mal treatment showed that multitype

mal treat ment is gen er ally asso ci ated with greater

impair ment than sin gle forms of child mal treat ment

(Higgins & McCabe, in press-b). Researchers have

now begun to high light the impact of multitype mal -

treatment and the impor tance of assess ing more than

one form of mal treat ment (e.g., Rorty, Yager, &

Rossotto, 1994; Sanders & Becker-Lausen, 1995).

Two major issues have been iden ti fied recently by

research ers: (a) the comorbidity of mul tiple forms of

child abuse and neglect and (b) the poten tial inde -

pendent or interactional con tri bu tion of fam ily fac

tors. How ever, even when research ers acknowl edge

the poten tial con tribution of other fac tors to the

adjust ment prob lems observed in mal treated pop u la

tions, they tend to look at only one of these two issues.

The con tri bu tion of the cur rent study is its dual focus:

the role of both fam ily fac tors and all forms of child

maltreatment to the adjust ment prob lems of adults. A

large pro por tion of the child mal treatment lit erature

is focused on the del eterious effects of sex ual abuse.

REZULTATI:

The results of this study sup ported the hypoth eses

that fam ily char ac ter is tics would pre dict both reports

of child hood mal treatment and cur rent adjust ment;

the degree of mal treating behav iors reported would

pre dict adjust ment; mal treat ment would pre dict

adjust ment after con trol ling for fam ily envi ron ment;

and that there would be a high degree of over lap

between reports of expe riencing sex ual abuse, phys i -

cal abuse, psy cho log i cal mal treat ment, neglect, and

wit ness ing fam ily vio lence. Paren tal sex ual puni tiveness

(the fre quency with which adults reported that

their par ents had responded puni tively or neg a tively

with regard to sex ual issues and behav iors) stood out

as an impor tant pre dictor of all types of child mal -

treat ment except for wit ness ing fam ily vio lence.

Expe ri ences of mal treat ing behav iors in child hood

were asso ciated with both trauma symptomatology

and self-depre ciation in adult hood. Sex ual abuse and

psy cho log i cal mal treatment were the types of mal -

treatment most strongly related to trauma symp tomatology

and self-depre ci a tion. These asso ci a tions

were still pres ent even after allow ing for fam ily back -

ground fac tors. Con sis tent with the find ings of some

research ers (e.g., Nash, Hulsey, Sex ton, Harralson, &

Lambert, 1993; Nash, Neimeyer, Hulsey, & Lam bert,

1998), this sug gests that child mal treatment is not just

an expres sion of a neg a tive fam ily envi ronment but is

also an inde pendent source of trauma with long-term

negative cor relates.

The results indi cated that there was a high degree

of over lap between adults’ reports of sex ual abuse,

physical abuse, psy chological mal treat ment, neglect,

and wit ness ing fam ily vio lence. Prob lems tend to

occur together. Children who are rid iculed or sub -

jected to ver bal attacks are also likely to be phys i cally

pun ished or harmed, have their phys i cal or emo tional

needs neglected, and wit ness vio lence being directed

toward other mem bers of the fam ily. Con sistent with

the find ings of Bernstein et al. (1994), all mal treat -

ment types were strongly asso ciated with each other.

Fam ilies in

which chil dren expe rience mal treat ing behav ior are

more likely to be char ac ter ized by paren tal sex ual

punitiveness toward chil dren, low fam ily cohe sion

and adapt abil ity, and poor qual ity interparental rela -

tion ships; how ever, paren tal divorce did not pre dict

child mal treat ment.

Studies from a variety of literature support the premise that avoidance

and escape behaviors play fundamental roles in the development

and maintenance of PTSD and trauma-related problems.

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy in the Treatment of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder -

SUSAN M. ORSILLO, SONJA V. BATTEN - BEHAVIOR MODIFICATION, Vol. 29 No. 1, January 2005 95-129

Proces prilagoditve v normalni situaciji, ki je za nas stresna

The restriction in the capacity for conscious information processing, and the greater prominence given to processing by an automatic, imaginal memory system, would appear to be advantageous. In normal circumstances, with the resumption of safety, flashbacks operate to transfer information from one memory system to the other, and rapidly decline in frequency after a few hours or days.

Proces prilagoditve v situaciji, ko reagiramo s PTSD

In PTSD, however, this process seems to be blocked, either because of too great a discrepancy between the contents of the VAM and SAM systems, or because the flashbacks are too aversive and have to be avoided. The absence of detailed verbally accessible memories prevents the inhibition of flashbacks, and high levels of negative emotions such as guilt and shame can, additionally or alternatively, prevent the inhibition of conscious thoughts and memories concerning the trauma. Further progress in developing what are already moderately effective treatments for PTSD is likely to depend on an integration of our knowledge about the phenomenology, underlying cognitive mechanisms, and neurobiology of the disorder.

Although some individuals who have experienced traumatic life events do not seem to suffer from acute or chronic psychological distress, most individuals do report a range of psychological symptoms (e.g., anxiety, fear, phobic responses, and depression) following a traumatic experience (Classen, Koopman, & Spiegel, 1993; Neville & Heppner, 1999). Research has indicated that factors that influence an individual’s reaction to trauma include the nature and severity of the event and the individual’s ability to cope with stress and trauma in his or her life (e.g., Snyder & Ford, 1987).

In fact, in the past 20 years, a substantial body of research indicates that applied problem solving and coping play a crucial adaptive role in dealing with stressful life events and often mediate or moderate the relationship between stress and both psychological and physical health (e.g., Heppner & Hillerbrand, 1991; Heppner& Lee, 2002; Heppner, Witty, & Dixon, 2004; Snyder & Ford, 1987; Summerfeldt & Endler, 1996; Zeidner & Endler, 1996).

(P. Paul Heppner and Mary J. Heppner, Development and Validation of a Collectivist Coping Styles Inventory, Journal of Counseling Psychology, 2006, Vol. 53, No. 1, 107–125)

POSLEDICE TRAVME – REVIKTIMIZACIJA IN KOMPULZIVNO PONAVLJANJE

Recent studies have noted a relationship between the experience of CSA and an increased vulnerability for assaults in adulthood (for review, see Messman & Long, 1996; Polusny & Follette, 1995). Several factors appear to heighten a woman’s vulnerability to revictimization. Factors such as learning processes (Wheeler & Berliner, 1988), denial (Roth, Wayland, & Woolsey, 1990), substance abuse (Briere & Runtz, 1987), low self-esteem (Finkelhor & Browne, 1985; Jehu & Gazan, 1983), dissociation (Sandberg, Matorin, & Lynn, 1999), sexual attitudes (Smith, Whealin, Davies, & Jackson, 1996), learned helplessness (Finkelhor & Browne, 1985; Peterson & Seligman, 1983;Walker&Browne, 1985), and choices regarding relationships (Jehu & Gazan, 1983) may contribute to increased risk for adult assaults.

Studies of revictimization with community women suggest rates of revictimization ranging from 37% to 68% (Gorcey, Santiago, & McCall-Perez, 1986; Russell, 1986; Wyatt, Guthrie, & Notgrass, 1992). In one such study, CSA survivors were 2.4 times more likely than nonvictims to be revictimized as adults (Wyatt et al., 1992).

Current findings do suggest that the revictimization of CSA survivors is associated with increased psychological distress.

Sexual revictimization has been explained as resulting from poor risk recognition in women who have been previously victimized (Breitenbecher, 2001; Gold, Sinclair,&Balge, 1999).

…studies suggest that women with a history of sexual assault may have difficulties responding effectively to sexual assault risk, rather than in recognizing it.

studies suggest that revictimization is the result of inappropriate or ineffective behavioral responses to risk rather than risk recognition (Meadows et al., 1997; Naugle, 1999). VanZile-Tamsen et al.

… women hold about sources of harm and their susceptibility to that harm determine how they interpret environmental stimuli and prepare to respond. In social interactions with male acquaintances (e.g., at parties or on dates), risk perception processes compete withwomen’s goals for entertainment, friendship, and intimacy, which dominate working cognitive processing. In these situations, sexual advances from a known perpetrator will likely be interpreted as sexual interest rather than aggression. Women may either miss or dismiss threat-related cues while focusing on having fun, finding a potential partner, or maintaining a current relationship (Livingston & Testa, 2000; Norris, Nurius, & Dimeff, 1996; Nurius, 2000). (The Impact of Sexual Assault History and Relationship Context on Appraisal of and Responses to Acquaintance Sexual Assault Risk 814 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / July 2005)

…findings offer support for previous research suggesting thatwomen are less likely to appraise a situation as risky when the perpetrator is known (Hickman & Muehlenhard, 1997; Nurius, 2000). Women are more likely toperceive rape-related threat when the perpetrator is someone with whom they do not have an expectation of sexual intimacy. This effect of degree of intimacy on behavioral responses is largely mediated through appraisal. Hence, by failing to recognize advances from an acquaintance as sexual assault threat, women fail to engage in direct resistance, which is most likely to thwart the advances. The Impact of Sexual Assault History and Relationship Context on Appraisal of and Responses to Acquaintance Sexual Assault Risk 814 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / July 2005)

- unexpected finding was that women are more likely to use indirect resistance when the perpetrator is a friend or date, regardless of the degree of risk perceived in the situation. The Impact of Sexual Assault History and Relationship Context on Appraisal of and Responses to Acquaintance Sexual Assault Risk 814 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / July 2005)

…this finding is consistent with the fact that behavioral responses are guided not just by risk appraisal or assertiveness but by concerns for the relationship, as well. Women in such situations may be ambivalent. On one hand, they may not want to engage in sexual activity; however, they may be focused on preserving the relationship or saving the man’s feelings (Livingston & Testa, 2000; Norris et al., 1996) and, thus, respond nonassertively and less effectively. The Impact of Sexual Assault History and Relationship Context on Appraisal of and Responses to Acquaintance Sexual Assault Risk 814 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / July 2005)

Another barrier to direct resistance involves the social embarrassment of accusing an acquaintance of sexual assault. Research has shown that people commonly blame the victim in situations of acquaintance sexual assault believing that the woman must have done something to encourage the man’s advances (Hammock & Richardson, 1997). The Impact of Sexual Assault History and Relationship Context on Appraisal of and Responses to Acquaintance Sexual Assault Risk 814 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / July 2005)

… results suggest that women can resist unwanted sexual advances through the development of assertiveness skills and effective resistance strategies; however, responses to acquaintance sexual assault depend on the interpretation of the situation according to schemata that are heavily influenced by relation- ship to the perpetrator.

Substantial empirical evidence suggests that women with a prior

experience of sexual assault are at greater risk for sexual victimization

than women without this experience. Across studies, rates

of sexual victimization in adulthood range from 28% to 38% for

women not sexually victimized in childhood, whereas childhood

sexual assault survivors’ rates of adult victimization range

from 48% to 66% (Banyard, Williams, & Siegel, 2001; Maker,

Kemmelmeier, & Peterson, 2001). Approximately two thirds of

adult victims of sexual assault report a history of earlier victimization

(Arata, 2002; Stermac, Reist, Addison,&Millar, 2002; Urquiza

& Goodlin-Jones, 1994). In a review of sexual revictimization literature,

Arata (2002) concludes that girls who are sexually victimized

in childhood are 1.5 to 2.5 times more likely to be sexually

assaulted in adolescence or adulthood than their nonvictimized

peers. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual

Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident,

and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530

research suggests that revictimized women

have poorer long-term psychological and emotional outcomes

than their singly victimized or nonvictimized counterparts

(Arata, 1999b; Banyard et al., 2001; Maker et al., 2001).

(ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual

Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident,

and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530)

Mostprominently, adult victims of sexual assault who have histories

of child sexual abuse have significantly higher levels of post-

traumatic stress symptoms than nonsurvivors or than women

with child-only or adult-only victimizations (Arata, 1999b;

Gidycz et al., 1993; Maker et al., 2001). (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident,

and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530)

Additionally, women victimized

by different perpetrators at different time points have

been shown to suffer greater levels of depression and anxiety

than women victimized only in childhood or only in adulthood

(Banyard et al., 2001; Gibson & Leitenberg, 2001; Gidycz et al.,

1993). Previously victimizedwomen take longer to recover froma

subsequent assault, experience more postassault PTSD symptomatology,

and use less effective coping methods to heal (Arata,

1999a; Gibson & Leitenberg, 2001). (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident,

and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530)

Revictimization may also exacerbate long-term sequelae such

as substance use and diminished physical health, although little

literature to date has specifically examined the impact of multiple

sexual assaults on these factors. Increased likelihood of alcohol

and other drug abuse has been consistently linked with childhood

sexual assault (Briere & Runtz, 1993). (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident,

and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530)

Numerous researchers have suggested that the sheer accumulation

of traumatic experiences is responsible for the increased

psychological distress found among revictimized women.

Women who experience sexual assault are at greater risk of experiencing

nonsexual traumas both in childhood and adulthood

(Banyard et al., 2001; Messman-Moore & Long, 2000; Stermac

et al., 2002).

Increasing numbers

of experiences of child sexual abuse, adult sexual assault, and

adult partner violence were accompanied by concomitant increases

in anxiety, depression, and posttraumatic symptoms. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530)

Alternatively, impaired psychological

functioning resulting from trauma may impede women’s

self-protective capacities in the face of later assault threats. Subsequent

revictimization then further erodes psychological wellbeing.

Forgetting or repressing a sexually assaultive experience

may play a role in this cycle.Women victimized by multiple perpetrators

were more likely to forget some or all of their initial

assault experience, and forgetting was a marginal predictor of

sexual revictimization. Forgetting may contribute to vulnerability

by preventing a woman from actively processing her experience,

from challenging self-blame or other self-defeating

schemas, and from developing coping skills that reduce vulnerability. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530)

it appears that earlier experiences of

sexual abuse create an initial vulnerability that is exacerbated by

subsequent childhood or adolescent victimizations.

(ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual

Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident,

and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530

The age at which initial sexual assaults occur and their accompanying

severity may increase women’s vulnerability by exacerbating

the psychological impact of an early victimization experience.

Evidence suggests that psychological distress, more

generally, and posttraumatic stress symptomatology (PTSD), in

particular, are likely mechanisms through which revictimization

vulnerability builds. PTSD-related symptoms have been shown

to moderate the relationship between early assault experiences

and revictimization (Sandberg, Matorin, & Lynn, 1999) as well as

the severity of childhood sexual abuse experiences and revictimization

in adulthood (Arata, 2000). Thus, for women with a

history of sexual victimization, high levels of current PTSD

symptomatology can exacerbate vulnerability, decrease selfprotective

capacity, or may constitute a vulnerability that potential

perpetrators seek out and exploit (Messman-Moore & Long,

2003)

(ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual

Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident,

and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530)

A complementary framework for understanding the link

between experiences of child sexual abuse and vulnerability to

revictimization is offered by Finkelhor and Browne (1985). These

authors theorized that the experience of sexual abuse damages a

young person’s self-concept and worldview through “trauma-genic dynamics,” which include a sense of betrayal, powerlessness,

stigmatization, and traumatic sexualization. Vulnerability

to reassault is posited to be exacerbated by psychological and

emotional impact consistent with these dynamics. Aspects of an

initial assault or its aftermath that intensify its psychological or

traumatic effect may therefore increase risk for revictimization

partially through the presence of traumagenic impact or previously

mentioned posttraumatic symptoms. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident,

and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530)

Finally, the presence of nonsexual trauma during childhood

can also increase young women’s risk of sexual revictimization. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530)

Thus, young people

whose early environments are characterized by risk of exposure

to multiple types of trauma appear to be at greater risk of increased

vulnerability. The nature of the environment’s response

to disclosures of abuse can also impact young people’s vulnerability.

Research has consistently demonstrated that supportive

responses to a child’s disclosure of sexual abuse are associated

with more positive mental health outcomes and more rapid healing

(Everson, Hunter, Runyon, Edelson, & Coulter, 1989; Gries

et al., 2000). Additionally, negative reactions fromformal or informal

helping systems have been associated with poorer mental

health outcomes following an assault (Filipas & Ullman, 2001).

The nature of and reaction to help seeking by victims therefore

appears to impact postassault functioning and, by extension, risk

of exposure to repeated sexual victimization. (ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530).

The results suggest that

singly victimized women differ from women with repeat victimizations

both in terms of the characteristics of their initial sexual

assault and with respect to the long-term psychological and

health consequences of their traumatic experiences. Both survivors

of ongoing abuse and survivors of multiple assaults by different

perpetrators were more likely to be younger at the time of

their first assault and to experience more severe initial assaults

(characterized by injury or degree of threat).

Within these differences,

a trend emerged in which victims of multiple perpetrators

were even younger and experienced a higher number of

severe aspects of victimization than women who were repeatedly

victimized by the same perpetrator throughout time. Additionally,

women victimized by multiple perpetrators were more

likely to have ever forgotten some or all of their initial assault

experience and to find the response of informal supports unhelpful

in response to disclosure compared to their singly victimized

counterparts.

Women victimized by multiple

perpetrators experienced significantly more nonsexual traumas

during their lifetime than either singly victimized women or

survivors of ongoing abuse. Additionally, multiply victimized

women experienced more types of sexually assaultive acts during

the course of their lives than women revictimized by the same

perpetrator throughout time. Thus, although both groups of

repeatedly victimized women may have more severe initial experiences

than singly victimized women, women hurt by multiple

offenders appear to face an added layer of exposure to both sexual

and nonsexual trauma.

This differential exposure to trauma is further reflected in the

findings related to the long-term outcomes of women with different

assault experiences. Consistent with expectations, women

victimized by multiple perpetrators were struggling with significantly

more current posttraumatic-stress-related symptoms,

more depression symptoms, and poorer self-rated health than

both the singly victimized group and the ongoing victimization

group. Additionally, the multiple perpetrator victimization

group was the only group of sexually assaulted women to have

significantly higher past month drug use than nonsexually victimized

respondents. Taken together, these results suggest that

although repeated victimization by the same perpetrator is associated

with greater psychological impact than a single assault experience,

a new assault by a different perpetrator may be a more

damaging form of revictimization than ongoing abuse by the

same offender.

(ERIN A. CASEY PAULA S. NURIUS,Trauma Exposure and Sexual Revictimization Risk, Comparisons Across Single, Multiple Incident, and Multiple Perpetrator Victimizations, VIOLENCE AGAINST WOMEN, Vol. 11 No. 4, April 2005 505-530).

Rather than

being independent traumatic events, a number of

studies have found that women with a history of child

sexual abuse are at increased risk for adult victimization

compared to women who do not report histories

of child sexual abuse (Ellis, Atkeson, & Calhoun,

1982; Fromuth, 1986; Miller et al., 1978; Urquiza &

Goodlin-Jones, 1994).

Women who have been sexually abused in childhood appear to be at greater

risk for revictimization as adults. In a community sample of 930 women,

Russell (1986) found that approximately 60% of the women who had been

abused in childhood also reported being raped or having experienced

attempted rape after the age of 14 compared with 35% ofwomen with no his-

tory of childhood abuse. A prospective study of 857

female college students conducted by Gidycz and colleagues (Gidycz, Coble,

Latham, & Layman, 1993) examined the relationship between childhood

sexual abuse and revictimization in adolescence and adulthood and showed

that childhood sexual abuse predicts revictimization in adulthood. A recent meta-analysis of 38 studies confirmed that women

with histories of childhood sexual abuse are more likely to be revictimized as

adults (Neumann, Houskamp, Pollock, & Briere, 1996).

In addition to these problems, previous research has revealed a link

between CSA and increased vulnerability for assaults in adulthood (Browne&

Finkelhor, 1986; Messman & Long, 1996; Polusny & Follette, 1995). This

phenomenon is called revictimization and may occur in the form of adult sexual

assault, physical abuse, or psychological maltreatment. Several factors

associated with CSA may increase a woman’s vulnerability to revictimization.

CSA may result in learned maladaptive behaviors, beliefs, and attitudes

and a failure to learn adaptive behaviors (Wheeler & Berliner, 1988). This

may result in inappropriate dating and sexual behavior, acceptance of rape

myths, and sex-role stereotypes in adulthood. Low self-esteem (Finkelhor &

Browne, 1985; Jehu & Gazan, 1983), learned helplessness (Finkelhor &

Browne, 1985; Peterson & Seligman, 1983), and relationship choices

(Jehu &Gazan, 1983) may also contribute to revictimization. Other psychological

difficulties associated with CSA may also be important. Both dissociation

and substance abuse have been linked to CSA (Briere, 1992). It has

been theorized that CSA survivors (CSAS) use substances to avoid unpleasant

affect and memories of the childhood abuse that may place them at risk for

subsequent assault (Briere, 1992; Polusny & Follette, 1995).

Studies with college and clinical

samples find that CSAS are more likely than nonvictims to experience

sexual victimization as adults (Briere & Runtz, 1987; Gidycz, Hanson, &

Latham, 1995; Koss & Dinero, 1989; Urquiza & Goodlin-Jones, 1994). One

study found that 32.1% of CSAS experienced adult victimization compared

with 13.6% of nonvictims (Gidycz, Coble, Latham,&Layman, 1993). Studies

of revictimization with communitywomen reveal rates of revictimization

ranging from 37% to 68% (Gorcey, Santiago,&McCall-Perez, 1986;Wyatt,

Guthrie,&Notgrass, 1992). Fergusson, Horwood, andLynskey (1997) found

that CSAS were 11 times more likely than nonvictims to experience rape or

attempted rape.

This studywas designed to overcome shortcomings of previous studies. First,

a well-operationalized definition ofCSAsimilar to those used by other researchers

and commonly used assessment measures to detect adult abuse experiences

were employed. A large sample was obtained with an appropriate comparison

group. The study of sexual revictimizationwas improved by examining unwanted

sexual contact with acquaintances and strangers as well as by investigating the

method of coercion involved. A dimensional measurement of unwanted sexual

contact (i.e., fondling, oral-genital contact, and penetration by objects in addition

to intercourse) also strengthened this investigation, as it is the first study to examine

these experiences. This study extends previous findings by examining all

three forms of revictimization simultaneously.

Evidence here suggests that revictimization not only occurs as

unwanted sexual contact but also as physical and psychological abuse.

Research is needed to explain howand why revictimization occurs.With a

few exceptions (e.g., Finkelhor & Browne, 1985; Polusny & Follette, 1995;

Walker & Browne, 1985; Wheeler & Berliner, 1988), theoretical conceptualizations of revictimization are lacking. Information regarding methods of

coercion, especially verbal coercion and use of alcohol and drugs as well as

physical force, may become important in developing such theories. Given the

results from this study, it appears that CSAS are more vulnerable to verbal

coercion or pressure from individuals in authority. This may be because the

experience of CSA has instilled fear of authority figures. Previous CSA may

have resulted in the development of certain coping strategies, such as “going

along” with uncomfortable experiences, as the adoption of these strategies

has in the past minimized physical harm. These strategies, however, may

actually increase the woman’s risk for additional victimization. Research

regarding the psychological adjustment of women who are revictimized is

also needed. One may speculate that revictimized women will experience

poorer psychological adjustment than nonvictimized women or women with

only adult victimization experiences (see, e.g., Follette, Polusny, Bechtle, &

Naugle, 1996).

Revictimization further increases the risk of psychopathology for survivors

of childhood abuse. Combat veterans with PTSD were more likely to

have a history of CPA than those without PTSD (Bremner, Southwick,

Johnson, Yehuda, & Charney, 1993). Battered women with PTSD had a

higher rate of CSA(but not CPA) than batteredwomen without PTSD (Astin,

Ogland-Hand, Coleman, & Foy, 1995). Survivors of combined CSA and

CPA who were subsequently retraumatized (by rape, domestic violence, or

criminal victimization) had significantly higher rates of PTSD than those

without a history of childhood abuse or with CSA alone (Breslau, Chilcoat,

Kessler, & Davis, 1999; Molnar, Buka, & Kessler, 2001; Rodriguez, Ryan,

Rowan, & Foy, cited in Rodriguez et al., 1998). Retraumatization of those

with a history of CSA as compared to traumatization only in adulthood has

also been associated with more dissociative symptoms, alexithymia, lifetime

suicide attempts, and interpersonal problems (Cloitre, Scarvalone,&Difede,

1997).

Sexual abuse in childhood is a major risk factor for later sexual

revictimization (Chu, 1992; Koss & Dinero, 1989) and may lead to greater

sexual problems (Wyatt, Guthrie, & Notgrass, 1992) as well as other trauma

symptoms (Koverola, Proulx, Battle, & Hanna, 1996). These trauma symptoms

among women sexually abused in childhood may be exacerbated by

recent life stressors, such as problems at work or in the family (Koopman,

Gore-Felton, & Spiegel, 1997). (Recent Stressful Life Events,Sexual Revictimization, and Their

Relationship With Traumatic Stress Symptoms Among Women Sexually Abused in ChildhoodCATHERINE CLASSEN

RUTH NEVO,CHERYL KOOPMAN,KIRSTEN NEVILL-MANNING,CHERYL GORE-FELTON

DEBORAH S. ROSE,DAVID SPIEGEL

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 12, December 2002 1274-1290)

For a substantial minority of women, however, revictimization occurs.

Numerous studies have found that women with a history of sexual victimization

are at greater risk for future victimization (Gidycz, Hanson, & Layman,

1995; Himelein, 1995; Humphrey & White, 2000; Nishith, Mechanic, &

Resick, 2000). It has been speculated that such prior victimization may

reduce a woman’s ability to appraise risk and to set appropriate boundaries

(Nishith et al., 2000). Indeed,Wilson, Calhoun, and Bernat (1999) found that

women with a victimization history exhibited poorer risk recognition (i.e.,

judging when a man’s sexual advances place her at risk).Women with a history

of repeated sexual victimization took longer to indicate potential danger

in an audiotaped date rape scenario. These women appear to experience a

delay in recognizing sexually aggressive behaviors that may pose a threat.

(The Role of Sexual Victimization

in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64)

Abstract: Lifetime trauma histories were ascertained for females with confirmed histories of

childhood sexual abuse and comparison females participating in a longitudinal, prospective

study. Abused participants reported twice as many subsequent rapes or sexual

assaults (p = .07), 1.6 times as many physical affronts including domestic violence

(p = .01), almost four times as many incidences of self-inflicted harm (p =

.002), and more than 20% more subsequent, significant lifetime traumas (p = .04)

than did comparison participants. Sexual revictimization was positively correlated

with posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms (PTSD), peritraumatic dissociation,

and sexual preoccupation. Physical revictimization was positively correlated with

PTSD symptoms, pathological dissociation, and sexually permissive attitudes. Selfharm

was positively correlated with both peritraumatic and pathological dissociation.

Competing theoretical explanations for revictimization and self-harm are discussed

and evaluated.

Research over the past decade has documented a prospective link between

rape and subsequent revictimization in short-term follow-up studies of adult

victims (e.g., Gidycz, Hanson, & Layman, 1995; Kilpatrick, Acierno,

Resnick, Saunders, &Best, 1997). The link between childhood sexual abuse

and subsequent victimization that occurs later in adolescence or adulthood is

less well understood. A growing body of research has documented associations

between childhood sexual abuse and subsequent sexual victimization

(see Messman&Long, 1996, and Briere&Runtz, 1987, for reviews; see also

Arata & Lindman, 2002; Chu & Dill, 1990; Kessler & Bieschke, 1999; Koss

& Dinero, 1989; Merrill et al., 1999; Messman-Moore & Long, 2000) and

between childhood sexual abuse and laterphysical victimization including

domestic violence (Arata, 1999; Collins, 1998; Gilbert, El-Bassel, Schilling,

& Friedman, 1997; Krahe, Scheinberger-Olwig, Waizenhofer, & Kolpin,

1999; McClosky, 1997; Messman & Long, 1996). Otherstudies have documented

higher rates of self-abuse or self-harm in childhood sexual abuse victims

(Boudewyn & Liem, 1995; Romans, Martin, Anderson, Herbison, &

Mullen, 1995; Van der Kolk, Perry, & Herman, 1991; Winchel & Stanley,

1991; Yeo & Yeo, 1993). Further, it appears that the co-occurrence of multiple

types of child maltreatment (e.g., sexual abuse, physical abuse, child

neglect) puts children at considerable risk for revictimization in adulthood

(Briere, Woo, McRae, Foltz, & Sitzman, 1997; Dutton, Burghardt, Perrin,

Chrestman, & Halle, 1994; Hillis, 2001).

We operationally define victimization

(either sexual or physical) as harm perpetrated by an outside source that

serves as a reenactment of the initial abuse. Self-harm, on the other hand,

implies a direct reenactment inflicted by the survivor herself and represents a

certain internalization of the trauma. Therefore, self-harm is not considered

a category of revictimization but will be studied as a separate and distinct

phenomenon.

REZULTATI:

Analyses showed that, compared to nonabused participants, sexually

abused participants were twice as likely to have been raped or sexually

assaulted, almost fourtimes as likely to have inflicted subsequent self-harm

(in the form of suicide attempts or self-mutilation), reported significantly

higher rates of physical revictimization (including domestic violence), and

reported a greater number of significant subsequent lifetime traumas than

comparison participants. When alternative forms of childhood maltreatment

were taken into account, childhood sexual abuse was a unique predictor of

self-harm.

Concurrent pathological dissociaton was shown to be predictive of physical

victimization when in the company with variables from several theoreti-

cally distinct domains. These results indicate that a persistent reliance on dissociation

as a coping mechanism can place participants at increased risk for

physical harm. Thus, victims who adopt pathological dissociation as the primary

defense strategy in adolescence or adulthood may be less able to engage

in self-protection when physically threatened. Dissociation has been thought

to be associated with suicide and self-injurious behaviors, and these results

confirm this association (Brodsky, Cloitre, & Dulit, 1995). Self-harm may

not be a direct response to sexual abuse but to the dissociative experiences

that result from efforts to cope with the abuse.

Results also indicate that being sexually active orbelie ving that sexual

activity is permissible can increase one’s vulnerability for physical victimization.

The incidence of self-harm in sexual abuse victims was quite dramatic.

Being sexually abused was, by far, the strongest predictor of self-harm even

when in company with other forms of child maltreatment.

Abstract: This study investigated the relationship between the severity of childhood trauma and proneness

to victimization in adulthood in a sample of 155 Australian women. A tendency for both violent

and nonviolent revictimization was observed. The classical “repetition compulsion” theory of

revictimization is less able to accommodate these findings than theories that implicate an effect

of childhood abuse on self-concept. Additionally, the factors of peritraumatic dissociation, coping

styles, and attachment styles were examined as possible intervening variables in the revictimization

relationship. The findings of the study suggest that some coping styles mediate the

relationship between childhood abuse and victimization in adulthood, whereas aspects of

attachment styles may serve to moderate this relationship.

Severe abuse and neglect in childhood can have diverse effects on functioning

in adulthood, but one effect that has received the increasing attention of

researchers in recent years is proneness to revictimization. That is, adults

with a history of childhood trauma appear to have an enhanced likelihood of

becoming victims of various types of violence such as rape and other sexual

assaults, physical assault, battery, and domestic violence (Cloitre, Tardiff,

Marzuk, Leon,&Portera, 1996; Messman&Long, 1996; Sappington, Pharr,

Tunstall, & Rickert, 1997).

The classical explanation of revictimization appeals to the psychodynamic

notion of a “repetition compulsion” (Freud, 1920/1955). According to this

account, people who fail to accommodate to a traumatic experience may be

subconsciously driven to reenact that experience in an endeavor to achieve a

sense of mastery over the original trauma. The theory can be criticized for its

imputation that if abuse victims actually “want to be revictimized,” the perpetrators

of violence against these people can be absolved from at least some

personal responsibility for their actions. The issue of apportioning blame

nevertheless has no bearing on the validity of the theory.

Although the repetition compulsion theory continues to have its advocates

(Chu, 1992; van der Kolk, 1989) contemporary commentators on revictimization

tend to interpret the phenomenon as overdetermined, that is, as reflecting

several distinct processes in varying degrees (Sandberg, Lynn, & Green,

1994). Among the processes that might be pivotal in this context are dissociative

mechanisms, coping styles, and attachment styles.

OBRAMBNI MEHANIZMI:

Dissociation is one of the most primitive of defense mechanisms. As children

develop, they tend to acquire more sophisticated coping styles by virtue

of which they may eventually be able to accommodate to past traumas. Thus,

child abuse survivors who have refused to dwell on their abusive experience

or who have positively reframed their experience do tend to be more resilient

(Himelein & McElrath, 1996) and thereby may be less liable to revictimization,

although the reverse might be the case for those who rely on an escapist

coping style (Proulx, Koverola, Fedorowicz, & Kral, 1995). A study by

Myall and Gold (1995), however, failed to find support for coping style as a

mediator between childhood sexual abuse and sexual victimization in adulthood.

Nonetheless, there is scope for examining this issue in relation to childhood

trauma and revictimization as more broadly conceived.

REZULTATI:

The sample of Australian women evidenced the revictimization phenomenon

in a relatively broad context, namely, in relation to a range of childhood

trauma and various instances of victimization in adulthood.

Abstract: Recent investigations of risk factors for adult sexual assault have focused on a varietyof behavioral

and cognitive variables, including victim risk-taking behaviors. In this study, cognitive

appraisals of riskyactivities, behavioral intentions to engage in risk-taking behaviors, and alcohol

use were examined in relation to future involvement in risk-taking behaviors and the incidence

of sexual assault in a sample of college women. At Time 1, 50 (26%) participants reported

a historyof sexual victimization and at Time 2, 16 (12.7%) reported new sexual victimizations.

Discriminant function analysis indicated that alcohol use and expected involvement in risky

activities at Time 1 were associated with new sexual victimizations at Time 2. Hierarchical

regression analysis revealed that alcohol use and expected involvement in risky activities at Time

1 were predictive of frequencyof involvement in riskyse xual activities at Time 2. The implication

of these findings for future research is discussed.

Routine activities theory provides an overarching framework for howcertain

behaviors may be associated with increased risk of assault. This theory

proposes that activities involving greater exposure to potential assailants are

associated with increased risk of victimization (Miethe & Meier, 1990;

Mustaine & Tewksbury, 1998). Moreover, risk may be greater in certain

social settings, such as bars, where alcohol is consumed and aggression is

more likely (Parks & Miller, 1997). Research has shown that a combination

of behavioral factors (e.g., alcohol use) and leisure activities (e.g., going to

the mall, eating out) are associated with increased risk of criminal victimization

among collegewomen (Mustaine&Tewksbury, 1998). Therefore, exposure

to potential assailants serves as a risk factor for future sexual victimization,

particularly in interpersonal contexts where a woman is likely to be

perceived as a vulnerable target (e.g., Parks & Miller, 1997). Although routine

activities theory predicts that exposure to “strangers” is salient, the application

of this theory to acquaintances is particularly relevant in the case of

sexual assault, given that the majority of sexual assaults are perpetrated by

someone known to the victim (Koss, Dinero, Seibel, & Cox, 1988).

In addition to risk of assault associated with exposure to potential perpetrators

in specific contexts, research has demonstrated that certain behaviors

and activities are associated with increased risk of victimization. Studies

investigating the phenomenon of revictimization have examined sexual risktaking

behaviors as potential risk factors. Theorists have suggested that sexual

risk-taking behaviors, such as promiscuous and unprotected sexual activities,

may increase risk for sexual assault (Koss & Dinero, 1989). Koss and

Dinero (1989) hypothesized that Finkelhor and Browne’s (1985) concept of

traumatic sexualization may explain increased sexualized behaviors among

previously victimized women. More frequent sexual activity, an earlier age

of onset of sexual intercourse, and a higher number of sexual partners have

been found to strongly correlate with sexual assault (Alexander & Lupfer,

1987; Koss & Dinero, 1989; Mayall & Gold, 1995; Wyatt, Guthrie, &

Notgrass, 1992). Furthermore, victims report greater involvement in sexual

risk-taking behaviors in comparison to nonvictims, including promiscuous

and indiscriminate sexual activities as well as unsafe sexual behaviors that

place them at risk for contracting sexually transmitted diseases (STDs) and

human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) (see Polusny & Follette, 1995, for a

review).

Alcohol use has also received considerable attention as a risk factor for

sexual victimization (Koss & Dinero, 1989; Muehlenhard & Linton, 1987;

Testa & Parks, 1996). Alcohol use has been found to correlate with risk for

sexual assault in retrospective studies (Koss & Dinero, 1989), to be more

prevalent among sexual assault victims (Stewart, 1996; Testa & Dermen,

1999), and to predict future victimizations (Gidycz, Hanson, & Layman,

1995).

REZULTATI:

Of the study variables, alcohol use and behavioral

intentions to engage in risk-taking behaviors were strongly related to new

victimizations and engagement in risky sexual activities. Alcohol use consistently

played a strong role in the predictive analyses andwas found to relate to

both the occurrence of new victimizations and frequency of involvement in

risky sexual activities. Moreover, the quantity of alcohol use was an impor-

tant factor in that newly victimized women reported more than 3 times as

many average binge-drinking days at Time 1 in comparison to women who

were not victimized during the study.

In addition, behavioral intentions to engage in risk-taking behaviors were

strongly associated with future risk-taking behavior, providing support for

the theory of reasoned action.

Substantial empirical evidence suggests that women with a prior

experience of sexual assault are at greater risk for sexual victimization

than women without this experience. Across studies, rates

of sexual victimization in adulthood range from 28% to 38% for

women not sexually victimized in childhood, whereas childhood

sexual assault survivors’ rates of adult victimization range

from 48% to 66% (Banyard, Williams, & Siegel, 2001; Maker,

Kemmelmeier, & Peterson, 2001). Approximately two thirds of

adult victims of sexual assault report a history of earlier victimization

(Arata, 2002; Stermac, Reist, Addison,&Millar, 2002; Urquiza

& Goodlin-Jones, 1994). In a review of sexual revictimization literature,

Arata (2002) concludes that girls who are sexually victimized

in childhood are 1.5 to 2.5 times more likely to be sexually

assaulted in adolescence or adulthood than their nonvictimized

peers. Additionally, research suggests that revictimized women

have poorer long-term psychological and emotional outcomes

than their singly victimized or nonvictimized counterparts

(Arata, 1999b; Banyard et al., 2001; Maker et al., 2001).

FAKTORJI, KI POJASNJUJEJO RANLJIVOST: Zlorabe v otorštvu- začetna starost, težavnost napada, skupna teža psihološkega pritiska

PREDICTORS OF SEXUAL REVICTIMIZATION

Factors such as age, severity, and mental health consequences

may linkwomen’s early victimization experiences to later vulnerability

to new sexual assaults. Age at an initial sexual assault

experience has received attention within the revictimization literature,

with somewhat mixed results. Some research suggests that

women who are first sexually assaulted during childhood are at

greater risk of subsequent victimization than women first victimized

during adolescence (Maker et al., 2001). Alternatively, child

sexual abuse can increase vulnerability to new victimizations

during adolescence, which, in turn, increases risk of exposure to

sexual assault in adulthood (Gidycz, Coble, Latham, & Layman,

1993; Humphrey & White, 2000). Other research has found no

effect for age at first assault (Jankowski, Leitenberg, Henning, &

Coffey, 2002). On the whole, it appears that earlier experiences of

sexual abuse create an initial vulnerability that is exacerbated by

subsequent childhood or adolescent victimizations.

Severity of initial assault experiences is also suggested to impact

risk of revictimization. Early victimizations characterized by

greater degrees of threat, force, and invasiveness may differentially

predict revictimization above the experience of sexual abuse

alone (Arata, 2000; Collins, 1998; Irwin, 1999). Furthermore, some

evidence suggests that seriousness of initial experiences creates

risk for more severe later assault experiences (Humphrey &

White, 2000). In a prospective study of college women, Gidycz

et al. (1993) found that severity of sexual assaults during childhood

and adolescence predicted the severity of revictimization in

early adulthood. Similarly, Mayall and Gold (1995) found that

narrower definitions of child sexual abuse, including only physical

contact forms of assault, were predictive of revictimization,

whereas more broad conceptualizations of child sexual abuse

were not.

The age at which initial sexual assaults occur and their accompanying

severity may increase women’s vulnerability by exacerbating

the psychological impact of an early victimization experience.

Evidence suggests that psychological distress, more

generally, and posttraumatic stress symptomatology (PTSD), in

particular, are likely mechanisms through which revictimization

vulnerability builds. PTSD-related symptoms have been shown

to moderate the relationship between early assault experiences

and revictimization (Sandberg, Matorin, & Lynn, 1999) as well as

the severity of childhood sexual abuse experiences and revictimization

in adulthood (Arata, 2000). Thus, for women with a

history of sexual victimization, high levels of current PTSD

symptomatology can exacerbate vulnerability, decrease selfprotective

capacity, or may constitute a vulnerability that potential

perpetrators seek out and exploit (Messman-Moore & Long,

2003).

RAZVOJ RANLJIVOSTI ZA PONOVNO SPOLNO TRAVMATIZACIJO

Pojasnjevalni model za razvoj ranljivosti na podlagi zgodnje travme

A complementary framework for understanding the link

between experiences of child sexual abuse and vulnerability to

revictimization is offered by Finkelhor and Browne (1985). These

authors theorized that the experience of sexual abuse damages a

young person’s self-concept and worldview through “trauma-

genic dynamics,” which include a sense of betrayal, powerlessness,

stigmatization, and traumatic sexualization. Vulnerability

to reassault is posited to be exacerbated by psychological and

emotional impact consistent with these dynamics. Aspects of an

initial assault or its aftermath that intensify its psychological or

traumatic effect may therefore increase risk for revictimization

partially through the presence of traumagenic impact or previously

mentioned posttraumatic symptoms.

FAKTORJI, KI POJASNJUJEJO RANLJIVOST: Prisotnost multiplih travm v otroštvu

Finally, the presence of nonsexual trauma during childhood

can also increase young women’s risk of sexual revictimization.

Revictimized women are more likely to report neglect or physical

abuse by caretakers in childhood, witnessing parental violence in

childhood, and physical violence by a dating partner during adolescence

than singly or never-victimized women (Banyard et al.,

2001; Collins, 1998; Stermac et al., 2002). Thus, young people

whose early environments are characterized by risk of exposure

to multiple types of trauma appear to be at greater risk of increased

vulnerability. The nature of the environment’s response

to disclosures of abuse can also impact young people’s vulnerability.

Research has consistently demonstrated that supportive

responses to a child’s disclosure of sexual abuse are associated

with more positive mental health outcomes and more rapid healing

(Everson, Hunter, Runyon, Edelson, & Coulter, 1989; Gries

et al., 2000). Additionally, negative reactions from formal or informal

helping systems have been associated with poorer mental

health outcomes following an assault (Filipas & Ullman, 2001).

The nature of and reaction to help seeking by victims therefore

appears to impact postassault functioning and, by extension, risk

of exposure to repeated sexual victimization.

LONG-TERM OUTCOMES OF REVICTIMIZATION

Almost as consistent as the finding that previously victimized

women are at greater risk of sexual assault is evidence that multiply

victimized women have worse psychological outcomes than

their nonvictimized or singly victimized counterparts. Most

prominently, adult victims of sexual assault who have histories

of child sexual abuse have significantly higher levels of post-

traumatic stress symptoms than nonsurvivors or than women

with child-only or adult-only victimizations (Arata, 1999b;

Gidycz et al., 1993; Maker et al., 2001). Additionally, women victimized

by different perpetrators at different time points have

been shown to suffer greater levels of depression and anxiety

than women victimized only in childhood or only in adulthood

(Banyard et al., 2001; Gibson & Leitenberg, 2001; Gidycz et al.,

1993). Previously victimized women take longer to recover froma

subsequent assault, experience more postassault PTSD symptomatology,

and use less effective coping methods to heal (Arata,

1999a; Gibson & Leitenberg, 2001). Diminished psychological

health appears to be connected specifically to multiple interpersonal

traumas, such as sexual assault; noninterpersonal traumas,

such as serious illness or accidents, do not generate the level of

psychological distress present for many sexually revictimized

women (Green et al., 2000).

Revictimization may also exacerbate long-term sequelae such

as substance use and diminished physical health, although little

literature to date has specifically examined the impact of multiple

sexual assaults on these factors. Increased likelihood of alcohol

and other drug abuse has been consistently linked with childhood

sexual assault (Briere & Runtz, 1993).

PRIMERJAVA ZNAČILNOSTI ENKRAT NASPROTI VEČKRAT SPOLNO NAPADENIM ŽENSKAM (s ponavljajočim ali z večimi različnimi napadalci)

The results suggest that singly victimized women differ from women with repeat victimizations

both in terms of the characteristics of their initial sexual assault and with respect to the long-term psychological and health consequences of their traumatic experiences. Both survivors of ongoing abuse and survivors of multiple assaults by different perpetrators were more likely to be younger at the time of their first assault and to experience more severe initial assaults (characterized by injury or degree of threat). These results echo the findings of previous studies that connect revictimization to initial assault severity and earlier victimizations (Arata, 2000; Humphrey & White, 2000; Maker et al., 2001). Within these differences, a trend emerged in which victims of multiple perpetrators were even younger and experienced a higher number of

severe aspects of victimization than women who were repeatedly victimized by the same perpetrator throughout time. Additionally, women victimized by multiple perpetrators were more

likely to have ever forgotten some or all of their initial assault experience and to find the response of informal supports unhelpful in response to disclosure compared to their singly victimized

Even more marked were the results related to the cumulative exposure to trauma throughout time.Women victimized by multiple perpetrators experienced significantly more nonsexual traumas during their lifetime than either singly victimized women or survivors of ongoing abuse. Additionally, multiply victimized women experienced more types of sexually assaultive acts during the course of their lives than women revictimized by the same perpetrator throughout time. Thus, although both groups of repeatedly victimized women may have more severe initial experiences than singly victimized women, women hurt by multiple offenders appear to face an added layer of exposure to both sexual and nonsexual trauma.

Within the multivariate framework in this analysis, only a younger age at the time of an initial sexual victimization and exposure to physical abuse in childhood emerged as significant predictors of sexual victimization by different perpetrators throughout time.

Thus, women whose early environments are characterized by both physical and sexual trauma

risks may be most vulnerable to subsequent sexual assaults. Additionally, the univariate relationship between membership in the multiple perpetrator victimization group and more unhelpful responses from informal sources of support to disclosures of an initial sexual assault experience suggests that these repeatedly victimized women’s early environments may be more likely to be generally unsupportive of young women’s safety

These findings echo previous research in highlighting the importance of early remedial intervention and attention to the presence or lack of supports in a young person’s environment.

Victimized youth in environments characterized by risk of exposure to multiple types of violence or trauma may be at elevated risk for revictimization and the compounding psychological impact of multiple traumas (Banyard et al., 2001; Follette et al., 1996; Green et al., 2000).

Recent work points to the impairing effects of early violence exposure on victims’ self-concept and identity health, which, in turn, carry cognitive consequences for subsequent psychological well-being and life functioning (Kellogg, Hoffman, & Taylor, 1999; Nurius, Casey, Lindhorst, &

Macy, 2004).

Definicija ponavljanja

For the purposes of this paper, repetition phenomena are defined as contemporaneously observed or reported reactions, manifested as behaviors, feelings, cognitions, memories, or physical sensations, expressed on their own or in combination, that involve some degree of reexperiencing of significant past events (e.g., intrusive reexperiencing of trauma, recreation of trauma, transference, recurrent dreams, and acting out).

Kompulzivno ponavlanje

Psychodynamic theory links contemporaneous reactions to formative influences, the origins of which may lie in the distant past and of which a person may have no conscious recall. Reactions observed in therapy, or reported by patients, may repeat in substantial detail the behaviors, cognitions, and affects associated with particular events. Some repetition phenomena are referred to as acting out, recreation, or reenactment. They may be manifest as simple momentary responses or highly elaborate sequences of reactions that recreate and reenact complex traumatic and developmentally disruptive experiences.

Ponavljanje in PTSD

These particular repetitions are recognised by DSM-IV (APA, 1994) and ICD-10 (WHO, 1992) symptom lists for PTSD but without reference to notions of acting out, recreation, and reenactment. For instance, a person may “: : : act or feel as if some aspect of the trauma were recurring : : :” (DSM-IV, B3), and may experience “: : : intense psychological distress at exposure to internal or external cues : : :” (DSM-IV, B4). These are valuable operational definitions for acting out, reexperiencing, and recreation under conditions where the precipitating trauma is known.

Ponavljanje travme v sanjah

In support of more individually tailored perspectives these authors refer to research indicating that the repetition of trauma in dreams takes at least three different and distinct forms: anxiety dreams with trauma-related content, traumatic nightmares, and traumatic reenactment. Each of these forms of dreaming is known to be mediated by different neurophysiological and psychophysiological processes (Mellman, Kulick-Bell, Ashlock, & Nolan, 1995; Shalev, Orr, & Pitman, 1993)

Anksiozne sanje

Patients with acute stress disorder or PTSD (APA, 1994) precipitated by a single recent traumatic event of moderate severity are likely to experience dreams containing images that repeat aspects of what happened. The phenomenology of these dreams is unlikely to differ significantly from anxiety dreams reported by individuals who do not have PTSD. Characteristically, such dreams do not waken up the dreamer; they occur during REM sleep phases; and dream episodes are recalled at the end of a major sleep period. Although dream images can be distressing, manifest dream content is amenable to interpretation, review, and discussion that promote insight and therapeutic progress as well as lead to a reduced frequency of repetitions over time.

Travmatične nočne more

Traumatic nightmares, on the other hand, share some of the phenomenological features of anxiety dreams but, in other respects, are quite different. These are terrifying repetitive dreams of reexposure to traumatic events. They are known to occur during both REM and non-REM sleep, and, in chronic PTSD, they are often persistent, intrusive, and treatment-resistant. Nonetheless, Schreuder (1996) argues that traumatic nightmares can contain symbolic representations of anxieties rooted in pretrauma and posttrauma phases of a person’s life. Typically, such anxieties

concern existential issues, especially those involving threat to life, threat of abandonment and death.

Podoživljanje travme med spanjem

Posttraumatic reenactment during sleep defines an extreme end point of the continuum of dream repetitions. It is characterized by a subjective impression of reliving traumatic experiences. Schreuder (1996) describes these repetitions as exact and explicit recreations of traumatic incidents. Unlike other types of dreams, they constantly recur in a form that is largely unaltered and unelaborated, and they appear to be impervious to the effect of time. Anxieties and sensory perceptions experienced during posttraumatic reenactment repeat the most evocative reactions originally provoked by initial traumatic experiences. Triggers for posttraumatic reenactment are typically unrelated to anxieties associated with earlier developmental life phases, existential threats, or current reality-based conflicts or worries. The initial trauma that is reexperienced remains an isolated experience that has not been processed, and the reexperiencing has no symbolic or other relationship with anxieties and conflicts in later life (Schreuder, 1996).

Dinamična psihoterapija, psihotravmatologija in ponavljanje

• As indicated above, the field of dynamic psychotherapy has a long and distinguished tradition of recognizing, both in theory and in clinical practice, the subtle, compelling, diverse, and compulsive nature of repetition phenomena.

• Psychotraumatology has, from a perspective of positivistic science, confirmed that significant past life

experiences do evoke reactions that repeat some aspects of formative experiences. It is also clear that conscious recall of precipitating events is not a precondition for repetitions, but some representation of the experience in memory is.

Manifestacije ponavljanja

It must be accepted that repetitions manifest as a compulsion to acting out, recreation, transference, reenactment, and dreaming share some phenomenological features but, on closer examination, they are revealed to be much more diverse than generally realized. Any therapist treating patients with PTSD, or its related disorders, has to be clear about the conceptual implications of the differentiating phenomenological features of reported repetitions, and to plan treatment accordingly. Similarly, researchers will do well to recognize that all repetitions are not the same and should not be studied as if they were.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PRIMERJAVA RAZLIČNIH TRAVM

During the past 20 years, we have learned how similarly harmful are experiences of

terror, violence, and abuse, whether they occur on the combat field or at home. The

field of family violence has gained much fromthe field of traumatic stress, and collaborations

between these two previously separate fields have yielded important new

answers, as well as new research questions. The field of traumatic stress is poised to

integrate, more fully than in the past, a variety of aspects of trauma such as social

betrayal, as well as outcomes of trauma such as depression, criminality, and physiological

harm that go beyond posttraumatic stress. The field of family violence has

much to offer in this process.

The battered

woman syndrome characterized for the first time the trauma symptoms of

women in battered women’s shelters (Walker, 1983). As understanding of

posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) among Vietnam veterans grew, it was

becoming clear that women exposed to violence and terror in their homes

responded in much the same asVietnam veterans exposed to the violence and

terror of war. As Herman (1992) explained,

Only after 1980, when the efforts of combat veterans had legitimized the concept

of posttraumatic stress disorder, did it become clear that the psychological

syndrome seen in survivors of rape, domestic battery, and incest was essentially

the same as the syndrome seen in survivors of war. (p. 32)

Multiple Abuse Experiences

Several methodological limitations exist in empirical efforts directed at evaluating

the impact of child abuse. The majority of studies target the correlates of

childhood sexual abuse, with only moderate research interest in the outcome of

physical abuse and psychological abuse (Claussen & Crittenden, 1991; Mullen,

Martin, Anderson, Romans, & Herbison, 1996). Additionally, the failure to assess

for the co-occurrence of various types of abuse or the impact of experiencing several

types of abuse concurrently is even more problematic. Those studies that have

assessed for the presence of multiple types of abuse (e.g., sexual, physical, and psychological)

have found that they often co-occur and that different types of abuse

are associated with unique patterns of adult symptomatology (Briere & Runtz,

1990; Claussen & Crittenden, 1991; Elliott & Briere, 1994; Moeller, Bachmann,

& Moeller, 1993; Ney, Fung, &Wickett, 1994;Wind & Silvern, 1992). For example,

when assessing the impact of childhood physical, sexual, and psychological

abuse on adult functioning, Briere and Runtz (1990) found that psychological

abuse was associated with low self-esteem, sexual abuse with dysfunctional sexual

behavior, and physical abuse with anger/aggression. In another sample, Mullen

et al. (1996) found trends for unique abuse–outcome relationships between sexual

abuse and sexual difficulties, emotional abuse and low self-esteem, and physical

abuse and marital difficulties. Additionally, all types of abuse were associated

with increased psychopathology, interpersonal problems, sexual difficulties, and

decreased self-esteem. Thus, both common and unique symptomatic responses to

child abuse may occur.

Investigators have also found that individuals who experienced multiple types

of abuse report greater symptomatology than do individuals who experienced a single

type of abuse (Mullen et al., 1996;Wind&Silvern, 1992). Although the results

have not been consistent across studies with respect to the pattern and magnitude

of symptomatology reported, it appears that different types of abuse, considered

separately and in combination, have differential impact on adult psychological

functioning. These studies support the necessity of concurrent assessment of multiple

types of child abuse.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – ZDRAVJE

Childhood victimization not only is prevalent but also is associated with

negative long-term psychological and physical health problems. Documented

psychological problems include posttraumatic stress disorder,

depression, anxiety, somatization, substance abuse, eating disorders, personality

disorders, and suicidal behavior (Beitchman et al., 1992; Boudewyn &

Liem, 1995; Briere & Runtz, 1990; Brown & Anderson, 1991; McCauley

et al., 1997; Miller,Downs, Gondoli,&Keil, 1987; Polusny&Follette, 1995;

Romans, Martin, Anderson, Herbison,&Mullen, 1995; Rowan&Foy, 1993;

Saunders, Villeponteaux, Lipovsky, Kilpatrick, & Veronen, 1992; Silverman,

Reinherz, & Giaconia, 1996; Thompson, Kaslow, Lane, & Kingree, 2000;

Wyatt, 1985; Zlotnick, Zakriski, Shea, & Costello, 1996). Poor social and

academic outcomes have also been documented among survivors of childhood

victimization (Kaplan, Pelcovitz,&Labruna, 1999; Polusny&Follette,

1995).

Twenty years following the abuse, female victims were significantly

more likely than were nonvictims to evidence aggressive behaviors

(Weiler & Widom, 1996), posttraumatic stress disorder (Widom, 1999b),

substance abuse (Widom & White, 1997), poorer academic and intellectual

outcomes (Perez & Widom, 1994), and personality disorders (Widom,

1999a).

Research also has focused on the long-term physical health outcomes of

childhood victimization. Most of these studies have been based on clinical

samples and thus cannot be generalized to the population (Drossman et al.,

1990; Harrop-Griffith et al., 1988; Kimerling & Calhoun, 1994; Lechner,

Vogel, Garcia-Shelton, & Leichter, 1993; Leserman et al., 1996; Moeller,

Bachmann, & Moeller, 1993).

These studies found that women who had experienced

childhood maltreatment (sexual abuse, physical abuse, emotional abuse,

emotional neglect, or physical neglect) had significantly higher median

annual health care costs, lower perceptions of their overall health, greater

physical and emotional functional disability, a greater number of physical

health symptoms, and a greater number of health risk behaviors than did

women with no history of maltreatment.

REZULTATI:

Using data from a nationally representative sample of 8,000 women, we

found that physical and sexual victimization experienced in childhood were

associated with several health problems in adulthood. Physical and sexual

childhood victimization showed similar associations to the health measures.

Both physical and sexual victimization in childhood were significantly associated

with perceived general health, serious injury, chronic mental health

condition, and drug use, but neither physical nor sexual victimization was

associated with chronic physical health conditions. Although physical victimizationwas

associated with daily alcohol use but not miscarriages or stillbirths,

sexual victimization was associated with miscarriages or stillbirths

but not daily alcohol use.

Several mediating variables may explain why childhood victimization is

associated with health problems in adulthood. One possibility is that the

association is mediated by psychological variables, such as depression.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA

There is an increasing recognition that sexually abused adolescents show a

heterogeneity of consequences (Bennett, Hughes,&Luke, 2000;Bal,VanOost,

De Bourdeaudhuij, & Crombez, 2003). Anxiety, depression, dissociative

complaints, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), anger, delinquency, and

sexual problems are some of the most reported symptoms in these adolescents

(Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993).

Not only the relationship of the abuser to the child but also the

functioning of the family can contribute to symptom variety. Considerable

evidence indicates that a cohesive, supportive family environment may serve

as a buffer against the negative effect of sexual abuse (Ray&Jackson, 1997).

Until now, little research focused on family functioning in families with an

extrafamilial sexually abused adolescent. The aim of this studywas to look at

differences in trauma-specific symptoms and family functioning in intra- and

extrafamilial sexually abused adolescents.

In a follow-up study of young adults abused or neglected as children, almost 80% of the sample failed to meet criteria for successful psychosocial functioning (McGloin &Widom, 2001). A longitudinal community study of young adult abuse survivors found approximately the same proportion meeting clinical criteria for one or more psychiatric disorders (Silverman, Reinherz, &Giaconia, 1996). Child abuse has been linked with some of the most severe and intractable psychiatric and social problems, including borderline personality disorder, dissociative identity (multiple personality) disorder, suicidality, substance abuse, sociopathy, and violence (Herman, Perry, &van der Kolk, 1989; Johnson, Cohen, Brown, Smailes, &Bernstein, 1999; Kluft, 1996; National Research Council, 1993).

The inner protector model provides an elegant explanation for a range of problems experienced by abuse survivors. The phenomenon of dissociative identity disorder (DID) is particularly instructive. More than 90% of North Americans diagnosed with DID report histories of child abuse (see Kluft, 1996, for a review). Because of the severity of their symptoms and the extreme abuse they report, "clinicians in the dissociative disorder field, by consensus, regard DID as the paradigmatic example of the psychological response to severe, chronic childhood trauma" (Ross, 1996, p. 16). Patients with this disorder experience a fragmented identity in which distinct personality states (alters), often separated by amnesic barriers, take executive control of their behavior (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). The three most common alter types are inadequate or confused protectors, terrified children, and persecutors who act out violently and drive patients to injure themselves (Putnam, 1989; Ross, 1997). These alter types closely match the three internal roles proposed here.

A common clinical symptom among abuse survivors is depression, characterized by low self-esteem, guilt, and intense shame (Ammerman et al. , 1986; Browne &Finkelhor, 1986). It is best understood, I believe, not as the expression of a single role but of interaction among all three.

Child abuse is also associated with disorders of dissociation in later life (Briere, 1992).

Generally speaking, theoretical, clinical, and empirical studies concur that men’s childhood sexual

abuse may present varied long-term outcomes. The three major ones boil

down to the following: (a) some of these men manifest in adulthood various

symptoms or disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder, behavioural problems,

or emotional problems (Boudewyn & Liem, 1995; Fondacaro, Holt, &

Powell, 1999; Gold, Lucenko, Elhai, Swingle,&Sellers, 1999; Holmes, Offen,&

Waller, 1997); (b) others manifest specific pathologies of a sexual nature, such as

pedophilia, adult sexual aggression, or other types of paraphilia (Lenderking

et al., 1997; Lodico, Gruber, & Diclemente, 1996; McCellan, McCurry, &

Ronnei, 1997); and (c) certain men manifest no major pathology in adulthood

(Finkelhor, 1990; Laumann, Gagnon, Michael,&Michaels, 1994; Okami, 1991).

It is important to note that the first two groups are not independent of one another

and may overlap.

Internalizing problems, such as anxiety, depression, dissociative complaints and problems related to posttraumatic stress disorder, and externalizing symptoms, such as sexual problems and anger, are among the most frequently reported symptoms (Bal, Crombez, Van Oost, & De Bourdeaudhuij, 2003; Wolfe & Birt, 1997). In their review of longitudinal and follow-up studies on child and adolescent sexual abuse, Kendall-Tackett et al. (1993) concluded that for one half to two thirds of all children and adolescents, postabuse symptoms decreased with time, whereas 10% to 24% of symptoms intensified. However, this pattern of recovery seemed to be different for different symptoms. In their follow-up study with sexually abused children and adolescents, Gomes- Schwartz, Horowitz, Cardarelli, and Sauzier (1990) found that anxiety problems tended to decrease, whereas problems of anger and sexual preoccupation seemed to persist or worsen. This is consistent with Mannarino, Cohen, Smith, and Moore-Motily (1991), who found that at 6- and 12-month followups, sexually abused children improved significantly on internalizing problems but not on externalizing problems. Other studies, however, did not find significant improvements in symptomatology with time.

Sexual assault is associated with psychological morbidity including depression, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and anxiety (Ackerman, Newton, McPherson, Jones, & Dykman, 1998; Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor, 1995; Ellis, 1983; Roth & Lebowitz, 1988; Ullman & Filipas, 2001). Long-term effects of child sexual abuse include suicidal behavior, personality disturbances, substance abuse, eating disorders, and revictimization (Brier & Runtz, 1987; Chandy, Blum, & Resnick, 1996; Dube et al., 2001; McCauleyet al., 1997; Wonderlich et al., 2001). (The Experience of Sexual Assault. Findings From a Statewide Victim Needs Assessment)

Although fewer than half of all chil dren exposed to sin gle-inci dent trau mas suf fer posttraumatic

symptomatology (PTSD symp toms) suf fi cient to war -rant a clin ical diag nosis of PTSD (Green et al., 1994;LaGreca et al., 1996; Nader et al., 1990), chil dren sub -jected to mal treatment (Cuffe et al., 1998; Famularo et al., 1994; Neumann et al., 1996; Steiner et al., 1997), dev as tat ing emo tional loss (Appelbaum & Burns, 1991; Green et al., 1994; Nader et al., 1990; Winje & Ulvik, 1998), or to the cumu la tive effect of mul tiple trau mas (Cuffe et al., 1998; Neumann, et al., 1996; Steiner et al., 1997) are at high risk for PTSD. Sev eral ODD etiologic fac tors, forexam ple, pov erty, fam ily con flict, and paren tal psychopathology (Biederman, Newcorn, & Sprich, 1991; Frick, Lahey, Loeber, Stouthamer-Loeber, 1992; Web ster-Stratton, 1996), also are risk fac tors for PTSD (Flisher et al., 1997; Green et al., 1994; Steiner et al., 1997). Our find ings indi cate that not only vic timization trauma in gen eral (Ford et al., 1999) but more specifically trau matic phys i cal and sex ual mal treatment are prev a lent among chil dren diag nosed with ODD and sec ond arily among chil dren diag nosed with ADHD.

Children with a diagnosis of ADHD were never theless at risk for past exposure to mal treatment trauma but less so than children diagnosed with ODD: 25% had been exposed to physical mal treat ment, and one in nine had been exposed to sexual mal treatment. ADHD is heterogeneous with regard to psychiatric mor bid ity, with most severe impair ment associated with antisocial families (Faraone, Biederman, & Milberger, 1995).

Child Mal treatment, Other Trauma Ex po sure, and Posttraumatic Symptomatology Among Children With Oppositional De fi ant

and At tention Def i cit Hy peractivity Dis or ders

Julian D. Ford (University of Con necticut School of Med icine), Robert Racusin, Cynthia G. Ellis, Wil liam B. Daviss, Jessica Reiser, Amy Fleischer (Dartmouth Med i cal School), Julie Thomas (Youngs town State Uni ver sity).

Research on the impact of sexual

violence, whether in childhood or later in the life course, suggests that sexual

assault is associated with a number of short- and long-term mental health

consequences including depression, PTSD, and substance abuse (Campbell

& Soeken, 1998, 1999; Cascardi, Riggs, Hearst-Ikeda, & Foa, 1996;

Jasinski,Williams, & Siegel, 2000; Ullman & Brecklin, 2002). Research by

Boudreaux, Kilpatrick, Resnick, Best, and Saunders (1998) suggests that

sexual assault is more strongly related to PTSD and major depressive episodes

as compared to other types of violent victimization. Violence, particularly

sexual violence, also increases the risk for substance abuse, alcohol

dependency, problem drinking, and alcohol-related difficulties (Clark&Foy,

2000; Downs & Harrison, 1998; Jasinski et al., 2000; Lurigio, 1987). For

example, Harrison, Fulkerson, and Beebe (1997) identified the earlier initiation

of substance use among victims who were children and adolescents. Not

only are adolescentswith a history of sexual abuse at increased risk formultiplesubstance

use (Harrison et al., 1997) but also girls who are abused are significantly

more likely to have alcohol or drug arrests in adulthood (National

Institute of Justice, 1995). Alcohol and drugs may be used by victims in an

attempt to cope with the trauma of violence, alleviating the symptoms and

anxiety associated with victimization, thereby increasing feelings ofmastery

and control (Banaji & Steele, 1989; Flannery, Singer, Williams, & Castro,

1998; Runtz & Schallow, 1997). Saunders et al. (1999) suggested that given

the higher rates of depressive symptomatology associated with violent victimization

alcohol abuse is potentially a coping strategy for those dealing

with the symptoms associated with various mental health disorders. This

suggests that victims may use alcohol and drugs as a form of self-medication

(Jasinski et al., 2000; Spatz Widom, Ireland, & Glynn, 1995).

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – PTSD

Empirical findings indicate that the avoidance criterion of the DSM-IV diagnosis of PTSD (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994) may be too exclusive. Across study groups, the percentage of participants meeting the reexperiencing criterion or the hyperarousal criterion is much greater than the percentage meeting the avoidance criterion (e.g., Kilpatrick & Resnick, 1993; Schutzwohl & Maercker, 1997). Thus, it appears likely that, by using the criteria of the DSM-IV, some people with otherwise genuine PTSD symptoms are excluded from the diagnosis because they do not exhibit the required three avoidant symptoms (Davidson& Foa, 1993b). Perhaps the most prominent definition of partial PTSD was proposed by Blanchard et al. (1994, 1995). According to this definition, partial PTSD is diagnosed if the minimum number of symptoms for the reexperiencing criterion, and either the avoidance criterion or the hyperarousal criterion are met.

Giaconia et al. (1995) have maintained that PTSD interferes with and impairs adolescent psychosocial functioning and places them at a higher risk for other psychological disorders. Anger, rage, absence of feeling, impulsive behavior, or attention problems are associated with PTSD, but these symptoms can be misdiagnosed and seen as conduct disorders, attention deficit disorders, depression, or dissociative disorders (Terr, 1991). Failure to accurately diagnose PTSD could interfere with successful rehabilitation (Cauffman et al.).

Pathological dissociation has not been examined adequately within the female juvenile offender population. Pathological dissociation in adolescents can interfere with the construction of a sense of self; it can also contribute to their failure to integrate their sense of self and their ability to integrate sexual, aggressive, and relational feelings (Armstrong, Putnam, Carlson, Libero, & Smith, 1997).

A diagnosis of PTSD emphasizes the unusual nature of a stressor followed by a pattern of distressing physical and psychological responses. Characteristic features of PTSD include reexperiencing the traumatic event, emotional numbness or avoidance, and increased arousal (American Psychiatric Association, APA, 1994). Since the diagnosis of PTSD emphasizes both the stressor and patterns of response, PTSD theoretically can occur in any person placed in similar circumstances, thereby alleviating individual pathology or blame (Ochberg, 1991; Walker, 1991).

Stressor Criterion PTSD may occur as a result of traumatic events that have either natural or human origins (APA, 1994). The traumatic event or sequence of events triggering a posttraumatic response is “... overwhelming, and dangerous to one’s self...” (Figley, Scrignar, & Smith, 1988, p. 113). Events arising from intentional human action tend to be more severe and destructive than those of natural origin. Events that are human in origin are characterized by deliberateness, negligence, or malice that destroys trust and security in human relationships (Green, 1990; Karl, 1989; Ochberg, 1991; Silvern & Kaersvang, 1989). Debate exists in the literature about classifying the ongoing, deliberate acts of battering of women by male partners as a traumatic event that may be responsible for the development of PTSD (Campbell, 1990; Figley, 1992; Kemp, Rawlings, & Green, 1991; Walker, 1991). Possible traumatic events noted for PTSD are a serious threat or harm to one’s life or physical integrity and prolonged physical or sexual abuse (APA, 1994). Yet, there is confusion with the stressor criterion in that the boundaries of a traumatic event are not always clear.

Researchers have been interested in the phenomenon of human responses to traumatic events since the beginning of the century. Over the past several decades, posttraumatic stress research has extended beyond combat veterans to include victims of rape or other violent acts (Burgess & Holmstrom, 1974; Saunders, Arata, & Kilpatrick, 1990), incest (Goodwin, 1985; Herman, 1993; Lindberg & Distad, 1985), child abuse (Green, 1985; Terr, 1990), and natural disasters (Frederick, 1985; Murphy, 1986; Steinglass &Gerrity, 1989; Weinrich, Hardin, & Johnson, 1990).

Goldberg and associates (1990) confirmed this assertion noting that PTSD was a normal response to abnormal stress and not a reflection of underlying pathology. Using a matched comparison group of 2092 identical twins who were combat veterans, Goldberg et al. found that the severity and length of PTSD symptoms, which ranged from 15 to 30 years in some instances, were related to the intensity of the conflict experienced. Furthermore, reexperiencing the trauma, through intrusive thoughts, nightmares, and flashbacks, comprised the most significant cluster of PTSD symptoms.

The frequency and severity of traumatic experiences was cited as a major contributor to PTSD symptomatology across several trauma survivor groups (Foy & Card, 1987; Gallers, Foy, Donahoe, & Goldfarb, 1988; Greenwald & Leitenberg, 1990; Kilpatrick, Saunders, Amick-McMullen, Best, & Veronen, 1989; McLeer, Deblinger, Atkins, Foa, & Ralphe, 1988; Solkoff, Gray, & Keill, 1988; Wolfe, Gentile, & Wolfe, 1989).

Many theoretical accounts of PTSD have emphasized the centrality of intense fear as the emotional experience associated with symptoms of intrusive memories, avoidance of event-related stimuli, and increased hyperarousal (Lee, Scragg, & Turner, 2001). Recent research on trauma has implicated shame in the development of PTSD symptoms for rape survivors, war veterans, and adult victims of violent crime with an abuse history (Andrews, Brewin, Rose, & Kirk, 2000; Jaycox, Zoellner, & Foa, 2002; Fontana & Rosenheck, 1994).

Although there is considerable variation depending on gender, race, and

the type of trauma, it has been estimated that 5–11% of trauma victims will

develop posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (7).

PTSD is an anxiety disorder including three symptom clusters: reexperiencing the trauma through nightmares, flashbacks, or intrusive memories; autonomic hyperactivity, such as exaggerated startle response, night sweats, and irritability; and avoidance

symptoms, including social isolation, restricted range of emotion, and absence

of intimacy in relationships (8).

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a recognized psychiatric experience following an external traumatic event (Mezey & Robbins, 2001). This diagnostic category describes common symptoms experienced by individuals highly exposed to an event involving death or injury, with resultant fear and helplessness. PTSD occurs more often in women, people with more direct, traumatic disaster exposure, and those with a history of psychiatric illness. PTSD is highest in populations exposed to extreme violence, such as prisoners of war, concentration camp victims, and crime victims, suggesting that disasters caused by human aggression cause more psychological damage than natural disasters.

Of the three categories of diagnostic PSTD symptoms, the numbness symptom cluster is particularly diagnostic of a more severe reaction: Feeling numb, experiencing emotional withdrawal, or avoiding all reminders of the event (McMillen, North, & Smith, 2000; North et al., 1999; Shariat, Mallonee, Kruger, Farmer, & North, 1999; Ursano, Fullerton, Kao,&Bhartiya, 1995). The remaining two symptom clusters include: (1) re-experiencing the traumatic event or uncontrollable intrusive thoughts about the event and (2) arousal symptoms, which describes a hyper-alert state exhibited by being easily startled, increased irritability, difficulty concentrating, or sleep disturbances. Both intrusionand hyper-arousal reactions have been found to be almost universal reactions

to trauma, beginning in the hours and days following an event and often lasting months, particularly following disasters (McMillen, North, & Smith, 2000; North et al., 1999; Shariat et al., 1999

Areviewof the studies of PTSD symptomatology in children finds that about

25% to 40% of children of all ages exposed to traumatic events fit diagnostic

criteria for PTSD, according to the DSM-IV (Fletcher, 1996).

(ALYTIA A. LEVENDOSKY, ALISSA C. HUTH-BOCKS, MICHAEL A. SEMEL, DEBORAH L. SHAPIRO Trauma Symptoms in Preschool-Age Children

Exposed to Domestic Violence, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 2, February 2002 150-164)

Furthermore, in this study, symptoms of PTSD were associated with

increased severity of the four types of violence. In particular, symptoms of

reexperiencing were positively associated with all four types of violence and

symptoms of hyperarousal were positively associated with all but the severe

violence category. These findings mirror studies on older children (Graham-

Bermann & Levendosky, 1998b) and battered women (Kemp et al., 1995)

finding that more abuse toward the mother is associated with higher levels of

trauma symptoms in both children and their mothers. Thus, it is not merely

the presence of violence in the parental relationship but also the extent and

frequency of it. The lack of association of avoidant symptoms with any of the violence types lends further support to the hypothesis that the avoidant symptoms

are not as reflective of trauma in this age group.

(ALYTIA A. LEVENDOSKY, ALISSA C. HUTH-BOCKS, MICHAEL A. SEMEL, DEBORAH L. SHAPIRO Trauma Symptoms in Preschool-Age Children

Exposed to Domestic Violence, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 2, February 2002 150-164)

Following traumatic violence, about one third of individuals develop posttraumatic

stress disorder (PTSD), a syndrome characterized by reexperiencing

the traumatic event, the use of avoidant coping strategies to cope with

traumatic memories, emotional numbing, and hyperarousal (American Psychiatric

Association, 2000;World Health Organization, 1992). Reexperiencing

of the traumatic event may present as recurrent flashbacks or nightmares

and intensification of distress when exposed to reminders of the event.With

avoidance, cognitive and behavioral strategies are used to avoid thoughts,

feelings, activities, or situations associated with the trauma. Attempts to

avoid external situations associated with the trauma may lead to a constricted

lifestyle.

As understanding of

posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) among Vietnam veterans grew, it was

becoming clear that women exposed to violence and terror in their homes

responded in much the same asVietnam veterans exposed to the violence and

terror of war. As Herman (1992) explained,

DEFINICIJA TRAVME

Clarifying our definition of trauma should be one of our primary goals for

the next 10 years. At present, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSMIV,

American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for PTSD drew heavily

on aspects of terror.We now know that few traumatic events that cause longlasting

harm involve solely or even mostly terror. Sexual abuse can be highly

terrifying, as in the case of most stranger rape, or involve virtually no immediate

fear for life, as in the case of children who are groomed by perpetrators

to view the abuse as acceptable. Social betrayal is a potent dimension of

events that cause harm (see Figure 1) and very likely to be present in all forms

of family violence (Freyd, 1996, 2001). DePrince (2001) found that the

amount of betrayal in an event was more predictive of most negative symptoms

than the amount of terror and fear. Evenwar combat may include significant

elements of grief, shame, and betrayal (Shay, 1994).

Overall, family violence is still highly stigmatized

and difficult to prove in court (American Prosecutors Research Institute,

2003; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986). Survivors’ experiences of loss,

betrayal, shame, stigma, and isolation have yet to be considered as rigorously

as terror.

Clarifying our definition of trauma will lead to a clarification and expansion

of the effects of trauma. A whole class of difficulties following trauma

have received relatively little research attention, including relationships with

siblings, extended family, partners, and children. Sexual and sleep difficulties

are common among survivors and may have implications for mental and

physical well-being (e.g., Maltz, 2001; Matsakis, 1996).

Finally, it is becoming very clear that victimizations are not unrelated, and

multiple forms of victimization must be taken into account when assessing

the impact of victimization (Finkelhor, Ormrod, Turner,&Hamby, 2004). In

a report on the victimization experiences of a national representative sample

of 2,030 children, 288 (14%) reported experiencing between four and six

kinds of victimization, and 118 (9%) reported experiencing seven or more

kinds of victimization (Finkelhor, Ormrod, & Turner, 2004). Victimization

kinds were defined as endorsing any of the items comprising the following

scales: sexual victimization, physical assault, property victimization, maltreatment,

peer or sibling victimization, and witnessing or indirect victimization.

Controlling for several possible confounding variables, the number of

different kinds of victimization predicted anger, depression, and anxiety

better than chronic victimization of any one kind.

Gender is one potentially very powerful risk factor for victimization. In

general, men are more likely to be exposed towar combat, nonsexual assaults

between strangers, and to be victimized in public places (Craven, 1997; U.S.

Census Bureau, 2003), whereas women are more likely to be sexually

abused, injured by an intimate partner, and victimized in a private home (Craven,

1997; Finkelhor, 1994; Straus, 2001). Thus, the scope of traumas assessed and the categories used to produce categories of experiences that are

added together to form a measure of multiplicity of trauma experiences make

a great deal of difference.

A recent community survey revealed a number of gender differences in

exposure to various kinds of trauma (Goldberg & Freyd, under review).

Women were much more likely to report having been emotionally or psychologically

mistreated by someone close as adults (approximately 40% compared

to less than 12% of men) and as children (approximately 30% compared

to less than 14%). Women also reported more sexual abuse in

adulthood and in childhood than did men. However, men were much more

likely to report having witnessed someone who they were not close to being

killed, committing suicide, or being injured, in adulthood and childhood.

Overall, women reported more events involving someone close to them, and

men reported more events that did not involve other people, and events

involving others who were not close to them.

Different kinds of traumas are associated with particular outcomes. Traumas

that involve high levels of threat are often associated with PTSD while

secretive, family violence is more likely to be associated with dissociative

symptoms (Freyd, 1996; Herman, 1992). Thus, although the number of kinds

of traumas may predict general mental health outcomes, exposure to particular

kinds of victimization may predict memory difficulty, dissociation, and

PTSD. To the extent that exposure to violence is gendered, and outcomes differ

by type of trauma, trauma-related psychological, social, and physical outcomes

will be gender related. Understanding gender may be highly important

for designing prevention and intervention strategies.

UNLIKE MOST OTHER DSM-IV diagnoses,

posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) requires a

specific, identifiable event to occur to qualify for

the diagnosis (American Psychiatric Association,

1994).

Of the characteristics of various

traumatic events, one that has been described

as important is the period of time over

which traumatic events occur (Baum, O’Keefe,

& Davidson, 1990).

When compared to singleincident

traumatic events, chronic traumatization

has been associated with higher levels of

PTSD symptoms (Herman 1992a). Chronic

traumatization is characterized by repeated exposures

to traumatic stressors within the same

overall context over time. In the case of

chronic traumatization, the environment contains

an implied risk of danger even when there

is no actual traumatic incident occurring (Smith,

Smith,&Earp, 1999). Chronic traumatization may be damag-

ing, not just because of the specific and repeated

traumatic incidents but because of the effects of

living in a state of constant danger (Baum et al.,

1990; Herman, 1992b; Smith et al., 1999).

PTSD is one of the most common negative

outcomes associated with histories of child sexual

abuse among child and adult survivors

(Neumann, Houskamp, Pollock, & Briere, 1996;

Oddone, Genuis, & Violato, 2001). Rates of

PTSD in clinical samples of sexually abused

children have ranged from 21% to 74%,with the

majority of studies reporting rates between 40%

and 50% (Rodriguez, Van de Kemp, & Foy,

1998).

The rate of current PTSD associated with childhood

sexual abuse (CSA) ranges from 70% to 73% in help-seeking samples to 6%

to 12% in the community (Rodriguez et al., 1998). Similarly, women with a

history of childhood physical abuse (CPA) are 10 times as likely to currently

have PTSD than those without such a history (Duncan, Saunders, Kilpatrick,

Hanson, & Resnick, 1996). Combined CSA and CPA may be more likely

than either alone to lead to PTSD (Schaaf & McCanne, 1998).

Accordingly, the American Psychiatric Association

(1987) defined traumatic events as incidents that are “outside the

range of usual human experience” and are of such serious magnitude

that they can be expected to be “markedly distressing to almost anyone.”

Within this general definition, particular forms of trauma can be

further distinguished. For instance, Wheaton (1996) suggested that

traumatic life experiences can range from sudden events (e.g., parental

loss and natural disasters) to events that are more chronic in nature

(e.g., repeated physical and sexual abuse and war combat). Traumatic

events can also be characterized by their scope of influence. For

instance, certain traumatic events, such as war or natural disasters,

affect entire groups of people simultaneously and thus can be considered

macro-level traumas. Other forms of trauma, such as exposure to

physical violence or parental loss, primarily affect individuals in isolation

and thus can be thought of as micro-level traumas.

Roughly a decade ago, the de˘nition of

a traumatic event was expanded in the DSM-IV (APA, 1994) to include

learning about unexpected death or threat of death experienced by a family member. Since this change in criteria, greater attention has been given in the

empirical literature to psychopathology in relation to the death of a loved one.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – PTSD – FAKTORJI TVEGANJA

This study examined the prevalence of trauma, PTSD, and dissociation between genders within the same setting among juvenile offenders and referrals for a court-ordered psychological evaluation. The findings from this study are consistent with similar studies, which examined trauma and PTSD in an offender population and found high prevalence rates (Burton et al., 1994; Cauffman et al., 1998; Steiner et al., 1997). These findings are also consistent with research conducted within the general population. In a study of two samples of 2,000 women in each sample, Duncan, Saunders, Kilpatrick, Hanson, and Resnick (1996) found that victims of childhood physical assault were about 5 times more likely to have a profile of lifetime PTSD (53% vs. 11.2%). Furthermore, victims were 10 times more likely to be currently experiencing PTSD (32% vs. 3.8%) than were nonvictims. Breslau et al. (1998) studied 2,181 people in the Detroit area and found a lifetime prevalence rate of 89.6% of exposure to one or more traumatic events. The conditional probability of developing PTSD subsequent

to the trauma was 9.2%, with PTSD being twice as high in women. Assaultive violence resulted in the greatest risk for developing PTSD, according to Breslau et al. (1998). In a similar study, Breslau, Chilcoat, Kessler, and Davis (1999) found that any previous exposure to trauma was associated with a greater risk for developing PTSD, and experiencing more than one traumatic event yielded a higher risk for PTSD. These authors also note that their data indicated that a trauma experienced in childhood did not place the individual at any greater risk for developing PTSD than did a trauma experienced later in life. Breslau et al. (1998) did suggest that trauma experienced in childhood could make one more vulnerable for PTSD if another trauma were experienced in adulthood.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – PTSD – KRITIKA DIAGNOZE PTSM

In DSM-IV-TR (American

Psychiatric Association, 2000), the PTSD diagnosis consists of

Criterion A, which specifies a preceding traumatic event and an

initial response; Criteria B, C, and D, which articulate clusters of

“symptoms”—otherwise referred to as “the disturbance”; and Criteria

E and F, which function to further delimit the use of the diagnostic

category. Criterion A reads,

The personwas exposed to a traumatic event in which both of the following

were present:

? the person experienced, witnessed, or was confronted with an event

or events that involved actual or threatened death or serious injury,

or a threat to the physical integrity of others.

? The person’s response involved intense fear, or helplessness, or horror.

(p. 467)

Bonnie Burstow 431

In this regard, note Kirk and Kutchins’s

(1997) revelations: “There are 175 combinations of symptoms by

which PTSD can be diagnosed” and “it is possible for two people

who have no symptoms in common to receive a diagnosis of PTSD”

(p. 124)

Following the recognition that posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) may follow

a variety of stressors, including combat, torture, incarceration, physical and

sexual assault, and life-threatening illness and accidents, there has been considerable

debate among researchers and clinicians about how best to characterize the

kind of event that may reasonably attract a diagnosis of PTSD. In the American

Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,

3rd ed.-revised (DSM-III-R, APA, 1987), it was suggested that such an event

would be outside the range of usual human experience and would be markedly

distressing to almost everyone. This formulation, known as the stressor criterion

or Criterion A, was an integral part of the diagnosis of PTSD.

Faced with criticisms that this formulation of Criterion A was insufficiently

precise, and evidence that traumatic events involving threats to life and health were

rather more usual than had hitherto been assumed (e.g., McFarlane&de Girolamo,

1996), the 4th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

(DSM-IV, APA, 1994) changed Criterion A to require that a person experienced,

witnessed, or was confronted with events involving actual or threatened death,

physical injury, or other threats to the physical integrity of the self or others (new

Criterion A1). In addition, it attempted to specify the subjective impact of the

trauma with greater precision in a new Criterion A2 by requiring that the person’s

response had to involve intense fear, helplessness, or horror (March, 1993). The

DSM-IV field trial confirmed a strong association between retrospective reports

of subjective distress at the time of the trauma and subsequent PTSD (Kilpatrick

et al., 1998).

The addition of this subjective criterion implies that, during exposure to a

traumatic event, individuals reliably experience certain intense emotions. This

may seem reasonable, but it would not be surprising if some victims reported being

numb or in a daze during the event, or reported absence of memory for the

event, which might be expected to attenuate emotional intensity (see also Kilpatrick

et al., 1998). Alternatively some events, such as motor vehicle accidents and some

physical assaults, may happen too quickly for intense emotions to be registered

at the time. A third factor to consider is the distinction between “primary” emotions

occurring at the time of the trauma and “secondary” emotions arising out

of subsequent cognitive appraisal, which could also act as potential risk factors

for the development of PTSD (Brewin, Dalgleish, & Joseph, 1996). For example,

there is recent evidence that both anger with others and shame, which may reflect

secondary cognitive appraisal, are also strong predictors of PTSD symptoms

longitudinally (Andrews, Brewin, Rose, & Kirk, 2000).

In DSM-IV-TR (American

Psychiatric Association, 2000), the PTSD diagnosis consists of

Criterion A, which specifies a preceding traumatic event and an

initial response; Criteria B, C, and D, which articulate clusters of

“symptoms”—otherwise referred to as “the disturbance”; and Criteria

E and F, which function to further delimit the use of the diagnostic

category. Criterion A reads,

The personwas exposed to a traumatic event in which both of the following

were present:

? the person experienced, witnessed, or was confronted with an event

or events that involved actual or threatened death or serious injury,

or a threat to the physical integrity of others.

? The person’s response involved intense fear, or helplessness, or horror.

(p. 467)

Bonnie Burstow 431

In this regard, note Kirk and Kutchins’s

(1997) revelations: “There are 175 combinations of symptoms by

which PTSD can be diagnosed” and “it is possible for two people

who have no symptoms in common to receive a diagnosis of PTSD”

(p. 124)

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – BORDERLINE IN DRUGE OSEBN. MOTNJE

Results indicate

that BPD cannot be distinguished from other personality disorders

on the basis of traumatic life events. The authors highlight the

findings that individuals with BPD were no more likely to have

reported childhood sexual abuse or adult victimization than were

individuals with other personality disorders.

Individuals with BPD were more likely to have PTSD than

non-BPD participants, but PTSD rates were actually higher among

individuals with paranoid personality disorder, suggesting no

unique relationship between BPD and PTSD. Path analysis supported

these results; direct and indirect relationships between BPD

and PTSD were not significant in the models tested. Path analysis

indicated that childhood abuse was significantly related to both

BPD and PTSD directly and to PTSD indirectly through a history

of assault in adulthood. In summary, the authors state that BPD has

no unique claim among personality disorders as a trauma-spectrum

disorder. (Trauma Is Not Specifically Related to Borderline

Personality Disorder, Briefings in Behavioral Science Volume 22/Number 3

March 2004)

The recent literature on the relationship between childhood abuse and personality

disorders (PDs) appears relatively restricted to borderline and antisocial personality disorders and has focused mainly on clinical or incarcerated

samples. Despite the importance of establishing the etiological association

between childhood trauma and these disorders, broader investigations

of PDs, particularly among more heterogeneous samples, are also needed.

Research on the correlates of child abuse, as reviewed, for example, in Briere

(1992) and Trickett and McBride-Chang (1995), has demonstrated a link

between childhood abuse and a wide range of psychosocial difficulties.

Against these findings we hypothesized that men with any history of childhood

sexual or physical abuse would score higher on a measure of PD symptoms

than men with no such history. Also, we hypothesized that men with

more than one type of abuse would score higher than men with only one type

of abuse. This would occur because of a greater frequency of abusive events,

some sort of synergy between the types of abuse, or the possibility that families

that create, or allow for, this level of abuse are more pathogenic in

general.

METODE, VPRAŠALNIKI

In addition to a brief demographic information questionnaire,

each packet included the Home Experiences History (HEH), a six-item

checklist of typically disruptive family situations, including parental abuse of

alcohol and drugs, physical and sexual abuse of a sibling, battery of a parent

by another adult, and removal from the home by child protective services.

The HEH was designed by Lisak and has been used in his research program

(e.g., Altschuler, 1997; Hopper, 1997; Lisak et al., 1996; Smith, 1997). The

Family Relationship Index (FRI) assesses perhaps more subtle aspects of

family functioning and is composed of the Cohesion, Expressiveness, and

(reverse-scored) Conflict subscales of the Family Environment Scale (FES), Form R (Moos & Moos, 1984). The FES is a widely used instrument, has

demonstrated construct validity in a range of applications, and has strong internal

consistency (Holahan & Moos, 1982; Moos & Moos, 1984). In what

seems to be a relatively common revision (e.g., Sines, 1984), the items were

recast in the past tense and participants were asked to respond in terms of

their family while they were “growing up.”

The Abuse-Perpetration Inventory (API) assesses histories of sexual and

physical abuse. The abuse items of the API list a series of potentially abusive

situations and ask subjects to respond regarding whether these happened to

them before age 16. For each item, if subjects answer positively, they are then

asked a short series of questions that assess the characteristics of the experience(

s). The situations are stated in strictly behavioral terms (e.g., “someone

had you fondle them,” “someone beat you with an object”); participants are

not asked whether they were “abused,” “molested,” and so forth. The API has

been used in six studies and has demonstrated validity with college male samples

(Lisak, Conklin, Hopper, Miller, & Smith, 1997).

Personality disorder symptoms were measured using the Schizoid, Antisocial,

Borderline, Narcissistic, Avoidant, Dependent, and Passive-

Aggressive subscales from the Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire-

Revised (PDQ-R) (Hyler & Rieder, 1987), an instrument designed to assess

personality disorders as defined by DSM-III-R criteria. The full PDQ-R was

not used primarily because of concerns about the length of the packet. In

addition to using Hyler and Rieder’s subscales, we constructed a composite

scale (PDQTOT), which is composed of the total number of responses indicating

pathology across the seven subscales. Note that results for PDQTOT

cannot be compared directly with other reports of PDQ-R composite scores

because of the deletion of several subscales.

KRITERIJI

The criteria for physical abuse consider the nature of the relationship, the

presence of injury, the threat of death, and the chronicity and frequency of the

experience(s). First, with rare exception, physical abuse is restricted to intrafamilial

relationships. Exceptions include relationships in which the other

party appears to have chronic proximity to, and power over, the subject,

beyond that afforded by the violent acts themselves. Moreover, the subject

must have apparently fewmeans of escape or recourse from the abusive situation

(e.g., a student-teacher relationship). Second, if the subject reports

physical injuries greater than mild bruises or scratches or thought, or was

told, that he was going to die, then the item is coded as abuse. Third, if these

injury criteria were not met but the event occurred at least 10 times and for at

least 1 year, then the item is coded as abuse as well. Note that the coding procedure

for physical abuse allows for aggregation of data across items. That is,

if a participant does not meet the duration or frequency criteria for an item but

if his responses would meet criteria if this item were restated to include subtypes

of physical abuse (e.g., kicking, hitting, stabbing) from another item(s) as well, then these combined itemswould be coded as abuse for the last of the

items aggregated (i.e., representing a total of one “item’s worth” of physical

abuse). This procedure was initiated to avoid categorizing as “not abused”

individuals whose physical abuse characteristics were not well represented

by the distribution of behaviors over the abuse items. In practice, however,

abuse designations based on aggregated items were infrequent compared to

those based on the more straightforward criteria.

Both hypotheses were supported. Compared to nonabused men, men with

any history of sexual or physical abuse indicated a greater degree of personality psychopathology associated withAvoidant, Borderline, Dependent,

and general PD (i.e., PDQTOT) symptoms. Moreover, men with both

types of abuse indicated greater symptomatology than men with one type of

abuse on PDQTOT, Borderline, and Dependent. In addition, Schizoid scores

exhibited trends consistent with both hypotheses. No differences were found

on the Antisocial, Narcissistic, or Passive-Aggressive subscales.

One interesting feature of these data is the absence of an association

between childhood abuse and symptoms of antisocial personality disorder

(APD). Although clinical levels of this disorder are less likely to be found in a

relatively high functioning college sample, this lack of any association whatsoever

was unexpected, given reports such as Luntz and Widom (1994) in

which histories of abuse and/or neglect predicted APD symptoms and diagnostic

status in adults when controlling for sex, race, age, socioeconomic

status (SES), and criminal history. The reason for this lack of significant findings

is unclear. One possible explanation is that DSM-III-R criteria (and

therefore the PDQ-R’s criteria) for APD are weighted in the direction of

criminality per se and away from personality characteristics such as callousness,

low frustration tolerance, lack of moral development, and so forth,

which are also associated with APD (Hare, Hart,&Harpur, 1991;Widiger&

Corbitt, 1995). Thus, this emphasis on antisocial behavior may be achieved at

the expense of sensitivity toward personality attributes, attributes that may be

of greater relevance when assessing a college sample. Yet this explanation is

weakened by findings that sexual or physical abuse histories predict antisocial

behaviors in community and college samples (Fergusson & Lynskey,

1997; Malinosky-Rummell & Hansen, 1993; Trickett & McBride-Chang,

1995).

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – SAMOMOR

Stressful events have long been acknowledged as important risk factors for

suicidal behavior. Although suicide research has generally focused on less

severe stressful events, a long-standing vulnerability for suicidal behavior may

be a sequela of prolonged traumatic stressors. The present paper discusses the

relationship between prolonged traumatic stress and subsequent suicidality by

reviewing studies that have examined suicidal behavior in relationship to child

abuse and combat trauma. Traumatic stress is conceptualized according to a

person-environment interactional paradigm, and this paradigm is used to

discuss the characteristics of traumatic events, recovery environments, and

individuals that may contribute to subsequent suicidality.

Research examining the relationship between traumatic

stress and suicide has primarily come from two areas of study: child

abuse trauma and combat trauma. Both child abuse and combat trauma

appear related to suicidal behavior, with the risk for suicidal behavior often

persisting for many years following the traumatic experience.

Stressful events have long been acknowledged as important risk factors

for suicidal behavior (Paykel, Prusoff, & Myers, 1974; Rich, Warsradt,

Nemiroff, Fowler, & Young, 1991). Both acute and chronic stressors are

related to suicidal behavior, and stressful events have an additive effect,

with the risk of suicidal behavior increasing as the number of stressful

events experienced by an individual increases (Adams, Overholser, &

Spirito, 1994).

Child abuse trauma and combat trauma differ greatly in populations

affected, events involved and adjustment demands placed on the individual.

However, they are similar in that both frequently represent Type II trauma

(Terr, 1992), a form of trauma that is prolonged and repeated (Herman,

1992).

Traumatic stress is a difficult construct to operationalize (Escobar,

1987; Lindy, Green, & Grace, 1987). Previous definitions of trauma focused

on the traumatic event as being "outside the range of usual human experience"

(American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1987), implying a categorical

difference between traumatic events and less severe stressful events

(Breslau & Davis, 1987). Recent definitions of trauma are more consistent

with theoretical models of stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) in emphasizing

the interaction between person and environment. Currently (APA, 1994),

the essence of a traumatic stressor is in the threatening nature of the event

and in its ability to overwhelm normal human adaptive capacities.

Physical and sexual abuse are acknowledged childhood traumatic

events. The potential for suicidal behavior has been observed among child

abuse survivors in clinical settings, with survivors often reporting a chronic

preoccupation with death and dying that accompanies feelings of helplessness,

hopelessness, and anger at self and others (Briere, 1989; Courtois,

1988). In research, a relationship between a history of child abuse trauma

and subsequent suicidal behavior has been observed in a variety of populations.

In a study with 1,040 psychiatric inpatients (Brown & Anderson, 1991),

suicidality was the most common admitting symptom for those patients who

had experienced child abuse trauma. Seventy five percent of the child abuse

survivors presented with suicidal behavior, compared with 57% of the nonabused

patients. A study with female psychiatric inpatients (Bryer, Nelson,

Miller, & Kroll, 1987) indicated a similar relationship between child abuse

trauma and suicidality. Patients presenting with suicidal ideation, gestures

or attempts were three times as likely to have a history of child abuse as

nonsuicidal patients.

The rate of suicidal behavior is also high among psychiatric outpatients

with a history of child abuse. One study (Anderson, Yasenik, & Ross, 1993)

reported that 49% of women seeking outpatient therapy for sexual abuse

reported having made one or more suicide attempts. In other studies, the

rate of suicide attempts among sexually abused women was 55% at a crisis

counseling center (Briere & Runtz, 1986) and 66% at a psychiatric emergency

room (Briere & Zaidi, 1989). In contrast, for depressed patients, a

23% to 35% rate of suicide attempts has been found (Adams & Overholser,

1992; Tanney, 1992).

Among college students, both suicidal ideation and suicide attempts

have been shown to be related to a history of child abuse. Suicidal ideation

was a coping mechanism self-reported by college students who experienced

sexual abuse as children (Runtz, 1993), and suicide attempts were more

likely given a history of physical abuse (Briere & Runtz, 1987) and sexual

abuse (Sedney & Brooks, 1984).

"Chronic suicidality" refers to recurrent suicidal episodes rather than

a persistent suicidal state (Motto, 1992). Chronic suicidality among trauma

survivors is described in clinical reports, with some survivors tracing their

suicidal preoccupation to their first incident of child abuse or to their combat

experience (Briere, 1989; Hendin & Haas, 1984). The chronicity of suicidality

following child abuse or combat trauma has not been directly examined

in empirical studies, but indirect research evidence suggests a long-term vulnerability

to suicidal behavior following child abuse or combat trauma.

Interactional paradigms of stress emphasize both environmental and

individual factors in defining stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Applying

this paradigm to traumatic stress, the factors defining a traumatic stressor

would include characteristics of: (1) the event, (2) the recovery environment

following the event, and (3) the individual (Wilson, 1989). Considering

these components separately could help clarify the relationship

between traumatic stress and suicidal behavior (i.e., which characteristics

of the event and recovery environment contribute to increased suicide

risk).

An increased number of traumatic stressors is related

to a greater likelihood of negative outcomes (Bryer et al., 1987; Solkoff,

Gray, & Keill, 1986).

As stress theory is being applied to traumatic stress, the recovery environment

is increasingly being recognized as a key element in the traumatic

stress process. Stress theory incorporates the role of environmental

social support in the stress process and posits a stress buffering effect for

social support (Cohen & Wills, 1985). According to stress theory, social

support buffers the effects of a stressful event in two ways: (1) by intervening

between the event and the stress reaction, thus reducing the appraised

threat or (2) by intervening between the experienced stress and a

potential pathological process by providing solutions or facilitating adaptive

behaviors. Studies of less severe stressful events have shown that environmental

social support reduces the likelihood of subsequent psychopathology

and suicidal behavior (Kessler & McLeod, 1985; Overholser, Norman, &

Miller, 1990).

…9 stran se ne vidi! …suggested that an individual's response to a traumatic event occurs in

three stages: initial reactions, ongoing accommodation, and long-term

elaboration. At each stage, the individual interacts with various dimensions

of the traumatic event and the recovery environment (Wilson, 1989).

Dissociation may be one mechanism that underlies the relationship between

traumatic stress and suicidality. Dissociation as a coping mechanism

often originates with traumatic experiences (Spiegel, 1993).

Guilt may also be a mechanism underlying the relationship between

trauma and suicidality. For some individuals, feelings of guilt, shame, responsibility

and complicity may originate with a traumatic event and become

integrated into the experience of self (Briere, 1989; Courtois, 1988).

Both child abuse trauma and combat

trauma are related to multiple psychiatric diagnoses, with psychiatric

symptoms often becoming chronic or recurrent (Green et al., 1989; Newman,

Orsillo, Herman, Niles, & Litz, 1995).

Among the diagnoses most frequently named in relation

to suicidal behavior are depressive disorders, substance abuse disorders,

PTSD, generalized anxiety disorder, and panic disorder. Psychiatric co-morbidity

often typifies the clinical presentation of child abuse and combat

trauma survivors (Beitchman et al., 1992; Green et al., 1989) which may

further increase the risk of suicidal behavior (Beautrais et al., 1996).

Manetta (1999) examined the relationship between different types of abuse (partner abuse, childhood physical and sexual abuse, and rape) and suicidal tendencies (as a symptom of depression) among African American women seen at medical

and psychiatric facilities (N= 91). The most frequent type of abuse reported among those who were suicidal was partner abuse (24.2%).

In several studies, suicidal behavior has been found to relate to trauma

exposure. For example, Lundin (1984), in a study of familial morbidity following

50 cases of sudden and unexpected death, found 10% of the surviving

bereaved to have committed suicide. Kilpatrick, Best, and Veronen

(1985) found the rate of attempted suicide to be 8.7 times higher among

victims of completed rape than among nonvictims. In a study among former

WW II prisoners of war (POWs), it was found that 57% of POWs imprisoned

by the Japanese harboured suicidal thoughts and that 7% of POWs

under the Germans had attempted suicide (Miller, Martin, & Spiro, 1989).

Somasundaram (1993) reported that suicidal thoughts were present in 38%

of a group of 160 former POWs subjected to torture in Sri Lanka.

The principal aim of the present study was to assess (a) the prevalence

of PTSD and psychiatric comorbidity, (b) the incidence suicidal behavior

among refugees with history of exposure to severe trauma, and c) the possible

difference between the different diagnoses with respect to modes of

suicidal behavior.

In this study of 149 traumatized refugees, the prevalence of PTSD

among all cases in which a principal psychiatric diagnosis was established

was 83%. A significant overrepresentation of suicidal behavior was found

in the group of refugees with PTSD diagnoses in comparison with the no

PTSD cases. Among the traumatized refugees in our study (all diagnoses

included) assessed with suicidal behavior, 40% (30/74) had made at least

one previous suicide attempt.

Besides paramount ethnic and cultural factors which have been reported

as potential risk for suicide among refugees (Ferrada-Noli, 1994;

Ferrada-Noli, Asberg, Ormstad, & Nordstrom, 1995; Ferrada-Noli, Asberg,

& Ormstad, 1996; Ferrada-Noli & Sundbom, 1996), new epidemiological

findings have disclosed the highly significant nation-wide overrepresentation

of immigrants in the Swedish suicide statistics, x2(1, N = 10,225) =

44.7, p = .0001).

The idea of a principal impact of PTSD (and also of reactive posttraumatic

depression) in the pathogenesis of suicidal behavior among tor-

tured victims may find indirect support in investigations describing the nature

and prevalence of PTSD symptoms, since some of these symptoms

have been reported earlier as being clinically associated with suicide risk

factors. In the study of Basoglu et al. (1994) 'restricted expectations' was

found to be three times more frequent among tortured than among nontortured

subjects. Ginsberg (1989) regarded the level of the patient's feelings

of hopelessness the most frequent association with attempted suicide

in patients suffering from depression, and the previous early findings in

suicidology on a high correlation between sense of hopelessness and inability

to see into the future and future suicide (Cavanough, 1986), are also to be

related with the very formulation of PTSD diagnostic criterion C-7 (sense

of a foreshortened future) in DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association

[APA], 1994).

A preliminary finding on an association between the suicide method

contained in the patient's suicidal ideation and the torture method to which

he or she recounted have being subjected was reported by Ferrada-Noli

(1993) at a CTD symposium at the Karolinska Hospital.

The principal aim of the present study was to ascertain whether relationships

existed between the type of torture stressors and suicidal ideation,

the hypothesis being that the nature of the torture methods would be

reflected in the content of posttraumatic self-destructive ideation.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – DEPRESIJA

Research has demonstrated that youth who are sexually orphysically abused are at greater risk for developing depression. Although the association between depression and child maltreatment has been well documented, much less is known about the potential differences in the clinical presentation of

depressive symptomatology among these victims. The current study examines differences in symptoms of depression in adolescents based on differing histories of abuse (i.e., sexual

abuse only, physical abuse only, sexual and physical abuse, and no history of sexual or physical abuse), abuse incident characteristics, and gender.

The presence of depression in adolescence is a significant risk predictor for major depression in adulthood (Harrington, Fudge, Rutter, Pickles, & Hill, 1990). In addition, depressed youth are at a significantly higher risk for suicide, which is the third leading cause of death in adolescents (Brent, 1995; Brent, Bridge, Johnson, & Connolly, 1996; Centers for Disease Control [CDC], 2002).

Research has demonstrated that children and adolescents who are sexually or physically abused are at greater risk for developing depression (Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor, 1996; Kilpatrick, Ruggiero, et al., 2003).

Research has emphasized that CSA is a risk factor for depression (e.g., Hill, 2003; Kilpatrick, Ruggiero, et al., 2003; Mannarino & Cohen, 1996). CPA also recently has been recognized as a risk factor for mood disorders (e.g., Brown&Kolko, 1999; Clark, De Bellis, Lynch, Cornelius, & Martin, 2003; Johnson, Kotch, et al., 2002; Runyon & Kenny, 2002). Despite the consistent findings that there is a strong link between depression and child maltreatment, little is known about the specific role of child abuse in the development and course of depression or how differences in abuse history may be associated with the clinical presentation of depressive symptomatology.

In another study comparing symptom differences in children who had been physically abused, neglected, or who had no abuse history, researchers found that children who were physically abused were more likely to be suicidal than children who were neglected as well as the children who were nonmaltreated (Finzi, Har-Even, Shnit, & Weizman, 2002).

Several studies have examined the perpetrator-victim relationship with regard to sequelae of the abuse, often finding that intrafamilial victims suffer greater physical and emotional injury (e.g., Faust, Runyon, & Kenny, 1995; Fischer&McDonald, 1998; Ruggiero et al., 2000). It is thought that the violation of a trusting intrafamilial relationship contributes to negative sequelae as a result of the abuse, particularly PTSD.

Thus, related depressive symptoms (i.e., problems with sleep, appetite, and energy) may be especially prevalent in children who experience abuse by the hand of a caretaker or in the home.

In addition, endorsements of several specific symptoms differed among the groups. Particularly noteworthy is that guilt and thoughts of hurting oneself were endorsed most by female adolescents in either group (abuse or no abuse history), indicating that these are particularly prevalent symptoms for female adolescents who are depressed. Some researchers have begun to examine how differences in normal adolescent development between boys and girls may affect such differences in depression, such as differences in coping styles (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1994), which may help to explain why suicidal ideation is higher among girls.

Child Maltreatment in Depressed Adolescents:Differences in Symptomatology Based on History of Abuse

Carla Kmett Danielson, Michael A. de Arellano, Dean G. Kilpatrick, Benjamin E. Saunders, Heidi S. Resnick

Medical University of South Carolina

CHILD MALTREATMENT, Vol. 10, No. 1, February 2005 37-48

Depression

is often a co-morbid condition that comes with PTSD (American Psychiatric

Association, 1994) and not surprisingly this is common in many of the cases with

sexual problems following trauma.

Depression is a common correlate of childhood maltreatment as well. A

number of studies have shown increased rates of depression, suicidality, and

lowself-esteem inwomen with a history of CSA (for reviews, see Beitchman

et al., 1992;Weiss, Longhurst, &Mazure, 1999). Duncan et al. (1996) found

thatwomen reporting a history of CPA were two times as likely to have had a

major depressive episode in their lifetime and four times as likely to be currently

experiencing a major depressive episode. Bernet and Stein (1999)

found that histories of childhood abuse among patients who were depressed

were associated with earlier onset of a first depressive episode, more lifetime

depressive episodes, and greater comorbidity. Self-depreciation has been

linked to CSA and childhood psychological maltreatment (Higgins &

McCabe, 2000b). In addition, childhood abuse has been associated with anxiety,

borderline personality disorder, somatization, sleep problems

(McCauley et al., 1997), dissociative symptoms (Chu & Dill, 1990), and

interpersonal and sexual difficulties (Rumstein-McKean&Hunsley, 2001).

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – PSIHOZA

Several lines of evidence suggest an association

between trauma and psychosis (1). First, studies

have demonstrated a high incidence of trauma in

the lifetimes of patients with psychosis. Ross et al.

(2) found that positive symptoms of schizophrenia

are related to a history of childhood trauma (2, 3).

Abused patients are particularly likely to experience

positive symptoms, such as hallucinations

(4–6) paranoid ideation, thought insertion, visual

hallucinations, ideas of reading someone else’s

mind, ideas of reference and hearing voices making

comments. In a recent study, childhood abuse was

a significant predictor of hallucinations, even in the

absence of adult abuse (7). Secondly, in patients

with other diagnoses, a history of child abuse has

also been found to _co-occur_ with a high frequency

of auditory hallucinations and delusions. Childhood

sexual abuse has an impact on the later

symptom profile of patients with bipolar affective

disorder, increasing their vulnerability to experience

hallucinations (8). Individuals with posttraumatic

stress disorder manifest increased levels

of positive psychotic symptoms (9). Dissociative

identity disorder, which is assumed to be a disturbance

resulting from severe childhood abuse

(10–12) may present with a great number of

Schneiderian first rank symptoms, particularly in

the form of auditory hallucinations (11, 12). It has

even been suggested that of all diagnostic categories,

psychosis displays the strongest associations

with child abuse (13, 14). Thirdly, according to

Briere (15) childhood sexual abuse is the most

powerful predictor of later psychiatric symptoms

and disorders after controlling for significant

demographic variables. A study of adult outpatients

found child abuse to be a more powerful

predictor of suicidality than a current diagnosis of

depression (16). The more severe the abuse, the

greater the probability of psychiatric disorder in

adulthood (17, 18).

It has been suggested that the experience of

abuse may create a biological (1) or psychological

(19) vulnerability for the development of psychotic

symptoms, including sub-clinical psychotic experiences

such as low-grade delusional ideation and

isolated auditory hallucinations (20). In the general

population, childhood sexual abuse is related to

schizotypy, including perceptual aberrations (21)

which are 10 times more common in adults who

were maltreated as children (22). In both clinical

and non-clinical populations, the diagnostic group

with the highest rate of childhood abuse consistently

reported the most Schneiderian symptoms

(23). Thus, two previous studies found evidence for

an association between abuse and psychotic experiences

in non-clinical samples (21, 24).

For example, subjects who reported

abuse in the highest frequency category had an

estimated 30 times greater chance to develop a

needs-based diagnosis of psychosis compared to

those not exposed to childhood abuse. Less

frequent abuse was associated with an estimated

five times greater risk to develop a need-based

diagnosis of psychosis compared to those without

any exposure to childhood abuse, whereas the risk

was not increased in subjects who reported abuse

in the lowest frequency category.

The results of this study suggest that reported

childhood abuse predicts psychotic symptoms in

adulthood in a dose–response fashion. The association

between childhood abuse and psychotic

symptoms was robust and remained significant

after adjustment for possible confounders.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – ADHD

I want to include some necessarily rather brief thinking about Gary’s

ADHD diagnosis and its relevance to his early history. In ‘Wrestling

with the whirlwind: an approach to the understanding of ADHD’

(Orford, 1998), the author quotes research done by Perry and his coworkers

in 1995.Their approach was neuro-biological, and it has now

been proved that early experience does indeed have an effect on the

development of neural pathways and later brain functioning. They note

that the symptoms of ADHD are very similar to those that occur during

trauma: the hyper-alertness, the need to act quickly, to live in constant

expectation of danger to the exclusion of other thoughts. What they suggest

is that in a critical period in infancy some children experience trauma

which initiates an habitual automatic response, as though to external

threat. As they grow up, these children are hyper-sensitive to threat and

revert to ‘action stations’ in time of crisis. Babies may be exposed to

frightening experiences which cause them to become habituated to feelings

of threat. Perry’s . nding has been that children with ADHD have

established neural pathways on the basis of response to threat and trauma.

In other words, some traumatized children may develop ADHD. As

Schore (1998) also explains, early traumatic events which result in excessive

use of projective identi. cation and then dissociation become

imprinted in the right brain as primitive defence mechanisms which can

potentially affect the regulation (or dysregulation) of feelings throughout

the lifespan. (On being dropped and picked up: adopted children and

their internal objects, JUDITH EDWA RDS )

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – ODVISNOST

[pic]

[pic]

[pic]

[pic]

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – SAMOPOHABLJANJE

In examining the possible connections, researchers

have noticed that incest is often present in the history of selfmutilators.

However, conclusions often go no further than reporting

the correlation. Although the correlation is noteworthy, it

overlooks an important observation: Not all incest survivors

mutilate themselves.Nostudies to date have explored the interrelation

of variables that lead to an understanding ofwhysomeCSA

survivors self-mutilate and others do not.

Of those with sexual abuse histories, 17% had selfmutilated.

None of thewomenwithout a sexual abuse history had

self-mutilated.

Astudy of 45 incest survivors by de Young (1982b) found that

58% had engaged in self-injurious behaviors, all beginning after

the CSA.Of hospitalized adolescent self-mutilators, 56% reported

sexual abuse in a study by C. Simpson and Porter (1981). Studies

by Goldney and Simpson (1975) and Grunebaum and Klerman

(1967) noted the connection between self-mutilation and sexual

abuse perpetrated by parents. In the sexual abuse accommodation

syndrome described by Summit (1983), self-mutilation was

considered one of the adaptive methods developed to survive

and accommodate the secrecy, helplessness, and entrapment of

the abuse. More recently, self-mutilation by CSA survivors has

been conceptualized as a symptom of post-traumatic stress disorder.

In one study, 25% of CSA survivors meeting the criteria for

post-traumatic stress disorder also self-mutilated (Albach &

Everaerd, 1992).

Depression is one emotion often associated with the emotional

sequelae of CSA (Beitchman et al., 1992). Brodsky, Cloitre, and

Dulit (1995) found a strong correlation between depression, selfmutilation,

and CSA histories.

Incorporating many of the above features, the

diagnosis of borderline personality disorder is commonly given

to patients who self-mutilate (Briere&Zaidi, 1989; Grunebaum&

Klerman, 1967; Shapiro, 1987; Walsh & Rosen, 1988).

Parental perpetrator(s). Self-mutilators, in comparison to the

nonmutilators, were sexually abused more often by their fathers

(51.3% to 25.0%) and mothers (10.3% to 2.8%). A parental perpetrator

appeared to substantially differentiate between the two

groups and therefore was included in the log-linear analysis.

Frequency of abuse. Participants were askedhowoften the sexual

experience(s) occurred. Therewas space provided to answer both

a standardized response and a respondent-generated estimate.

Results of this question supported that there is a relationship

between increased frequency and self-mutilation. On average,

self-mutilators remembered 22 nonfamilial sexual incidents and

45 familial ones.Womenwho did not self-injure remembered 10.8

and 38.44 incidents, respectively.

Penetration. The group of self-mutilators experienced less vaginal

intercourse than the nonmutilators in both familial (12.8% to

13.9%) and nonfamilial (37.9% to 47.8%) sexual abuse. However,

slightly more self-mutilators experienced anal (12.8% to 8.3%) or

attempted vaginal (30.8% to 19.4%) intercourse. The presence of

vaginal intercourse did not seem to contribute to the distinction

between self-mutilators and nonmutilators. Therewas some suggestion,

however, that the presence of anal or attempted vaginal

intercourse may indicate a proclivity toward self-mutilation.

However,more of

the fathers of the nonmutilators had died compared to fathers of

the mutilators (14.3% to 9.5%). These results were the opposite of

the suggested findings; therefore, the loss of one’s father was not

included in further analyses.

The loss of one’s mother, however, yielded a different pattern.

During the participants’ childhoods, more mothers of selfmutilators

died compared to mothers of nonmutilators (7.1% to

2.4%, n = 4, combined). Also, the mothers of mutilators were

seriously ill or suffered injuries more often in a comparison to

mothers of nonmutilators (38.1% to 31%, n = 16 and n = 13).

In examining the possible connections, researchers

have noticed that incest is often present in the history of selfmutilators.

However, conclusions often go no further than reporting

the correlation. Although the correlation is noteworthy, it

overlooks an important observation: Not all incest survivors

mutilate themselves.Nostudies to date have explored the interrelation

of variables that lead to an understanding ofwhysomeCSA

survivors self-mutilate and others do not.

Of those with sexual abuse histories, 17% had selfmutilated.

None of thewomenwithout a sexual abuse history had

self-mutilated.

Astudy of 45 incest survivors by de Young (1982b) found that

58% had engaged in self-injurious behaviors, all beginning after

the CSA.Of hospitalized adolescent self-mutilators, 56% reported

sexual abuse in a study by C. Simpson and Porter (1981). Studies

by Goldney and Simpson (1975) and Grunebaum and Klerman

(1967) noted the connection between self-mutilation and sexual

abuse perpetrated by parents. In the sexual abuse accommodation

syndrome described by Summit (1983), self-mutilation was

considered one of the adaptive methods developed to survive

and accommodate the secrecy, helplessness, and entrapment of

the abuse. More recently, self-mutilation by CSA survivors has

been conceptualized as a symptom of post-traumatic stress disorder.

In one study, 25% of CSA survivors meeting the criteria for

post-traumatic stress disorder also self-mutilated (Albach &

Everaerd, 1992).

Depression is one emotion often associated with the emotional

sequelae of CSA (Beitchman et al., 1992). Brodsky, Cloitre, and

Dulit (1995) found a strong correlation between depression, selfmutilation,

and CSA histories.

Incorporating many of the above features, the

diagnosis of borderline personality disorder is commonly given

to patients who self-mutilate (Briere&Zaidi, 1989; Grunebaum&

Klerman, 1967; Shapiro, 1987; Walsh & Rosen, 1988).

Over the past two decades, the study of CSAwithin the family

and its sequelae has been recognized as a significant issue for

many women. Estimates vary as to its prevalence, ranging from

about one third (31%) (Russell, 1983) (33%) (Wheeler & Walton,

1987) to 16% (Russell, 1983, 1984; Sedney & Brooks, 1984)

REZULTATI

Duration. The average duration of the CSA for the mutilators

was almost double that of the nonmutilators. With a range of

duration from less than 1 year to 22 years, the mutilators’ CSA

began at the mean age of 6.06 years (SD = 3.91) and stopped at the

mean age of 13.33 years (SD = 5.87). For the nonmutilators, the

CSA started at the mean age of 7.64 years (SD = 4.30), and the

mean age when it ended was 11.66 (SD = 6.36). Duration ranged

from less than 1 year to 34 years. Duration of the CSAappeared to

differentiate between the two groups and was included as a

potential variable in the model-building phase of the analysis.

Parental perpetrator(s). Self-mutilators, in comparison to the

nonmutilators, were sexually abused more often by their fathers

(51.3% to 25.0%) and mothers (10.3% to 2.8%). A parental perpetrator

appeared to substantially differentiate between the two

groups and therefore was included in the log-linear analysis.

Frequency of abuse. Participants were askedhowoften the sexual

experience(s) occurred. Therewas space provided to answer both

a standardized response and a respondent-generated estimate.

Results of this question supported that there is a relationship

between increased frequency and self-mutilation. On average,

self-mutilators remembered 22 nonfamilial sexual incidents and

45 familial ones.Womenwho did not self-injure remembered 10.8

and 38.44 incidents, respectively.

Penetration. The group of self-mutilators experienced less vaginal

intercourse than the nonmutilators in both familial (12.8% to

13.9%) and nonfamilial (37.9% to 47.8%) sexual abuse. However,

slightly more self-mutilators experienced anal (12.8% to 8.3%) or

attempted vaginal (30.8% to 19.4%) intercourse. The presence of

vaginal intercourse did not seem to contribute to the distinction

between self-mutilators and nonmutilators. Therewas some suggestion,

however, that the presence of anal or attempted vaginal

intercourse may indicate a proclivity toward self-mutilation.

However,more of

the fathers of the nonmutilators had died compared to fathers of

the mutilators (14.3% to 9.5%). These results were the opposite of

the suggested findings; therefore, the loss of one’s father was not

included in further analyses.

The loss of one’s mother, however, yielded a different pattern.

During the participants’ childhoods, more mothers of selfmutilators

died compared to mothers of nonmutilators (7.1% to

2.4%, n = 4, combined). Also, the mothers of mutilators were

seriously ill or suffered injuries more often in a comparison to

mothers of nonmutilators (38.1% to 31%, n = 16 and n = 13).

The nature

of the abuse, the relationship of the perpetrator,

the age when the abuse occurred, and the duration of

the abuse are variables of the abuse that further influence

the likelihood of self-harm.

Little has been written about the psychological determinants

that lead to self-harm as an expression of

the internal distress associated with a history of abuse.

Themore extreme forms of self-harm are defined as purposeful

actions that harm the body and that are outside

the bounds of social acceptability. These include cutting,

burning, abrading, or hitting oneself, inserting

sharp objects in the anus or vagina, pulling out body

hair, or other self-attacking behaviors that are idiosyncratic

to the survivor and his or her abuse history.

However, cutting has been found to be the most common

form of self-harm (Babicker & Arnold). Self-harm

is often an attempt to communicate and relieve pain

and maintain discourse.

Women who self-harm

often hate their bodies and consider their bodies to be

representations of their internalized badness and ugliness.

Cutting the external body symbolically attacks the

internal badness and, because of boundary confusion,

may represent an attack on the abuser.

Cutting, unlike childhood abuse, is within the control

of the trauma survivor. Of course, the relief it brings is

short-lived and often leads to shame and guilt, and the

cycle of pain, relief, and shame starts again.

Abstract: Lifetime trauma histories were ascertained for females with confirmed histories of

childhood sexual abuse and comparison females participating in a longitudinal, prospective

study. Abused participants reported twice as many subsequent rapes or sexual

assaults (p = .07), 1.6 times as many physical affronts including domestic violence

(p = .01), almost four times as many incidences of self-inflicted harm (p =

.002), and more than 20% more subsequent, significant lifetime traumas (p = .04)

than did comparison participants. Sexual revictimization was positively correlated

with posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms (PTSD), peritraumatic dissociation,

and sexual preoccupation. Physical revictimization was positively correlated with

PTSD symptoms, pathological dissociation, and sexually permissive attitudes. Selfharm

was positively correlated with both peritraumatic and pathological dissociation.

Competing theoretical explanations for revictimization and self-harm are discussed

and evaluated.

Research over the past decade has documented a prospective link between

rape and subsequent revictimization in short-term follow-up studies of adult

victims (e.g., Gidycz, Hanson, & Layman, 1995; Kilpatrick, Acierno,

Resnick, Saunders, &Best, 1997). The link between childhood sexual abuse

and subsequent victimization that occurs later in adolescence or adulthood is

less well understood. A growing body of research has documented associations

between childhood sexual abuse and subsequent sexual victimization

(see Messman&Long, 1996, and Briere&Runtz, 1987, for reviews; see also

Arata & Lindman, 2002; Chu & Dill, 1990; Kessler & Bieschke, 1999; Koss

& Dinero, 1989; Merrill et al., 1999; Messman-Moore & Long, 2000) and

between childhood sexual abuse and laterphysical victimization including

domestic violence (Arata, 1999; Collins, 1998; Gilbert, El-Bassel, Schilling,

& Friedman, 1997; Krahe, Scheinberger-Olwig, Waizenhofer, & Kolpin,

1999; McClosky, 1997; Messman & Long, 1996). Otherstudies have documented

higher rates of self-abuse or self-harm in childhood sexual abuse victims

(Boudewyn & Liem, 1995; Romans, Martin, Anderson, Herbison, &

Mullen, 1995; Van der Kolk, Perry, & Herman, 1991; Winchel & Stanley,

1991; Yeo & Yeo, 1993). Further, it appears that the co-occurrence of multiple

types of child maltreatment (e.g., sexual abuse, physical abuse, child

neglect) puts children at considerable risk for revictimization in adulthood

(Briere, Woo, McRae, Foltz, & Sitzman, 1997; Dutton, Burghardt, Perrin,

Chrestman, & Halle, 1994; Hillis, 2001).

We operationally define victimization

(either sexual or physical) as harm perpetrated by an outside source that

serves as a reenactment of the initial abuse. Self-harm, on the other hand,

implies a direct reenactment inflicted by the survivor herself and represents a

certain internalization of the trauma. Therefore, self-harm is not considered

a category of revictimization but will be studied as a separate and distinct

phenomenon.

REZULTATI:

Analyses showed that, compared to nonabused participants, sexually

abused participants were twice as likely to have been raped or sexually

assaulted, almost fourtimes as likely to have inflicted subsequent self-harm

(in the form of suicide attempts or self-mutilation), reported significantly

higher rates of physical revictimization (including domestic violence), and

reported a greater number of significant subsequent lifetime traumas than

comparison participants. When alternative forms of childhood maltreatment

were taken into account, childhood sexual abuse was a unique predictor of

self-harm.

Concurrent pathological dissociaton was shown to be predictive of physical

victimization when in the company with variables from several theoreti-

cally distinct domains. These results indicate that a persistent reliance on dissociation

as a coping mechanism can place participants at increased risk for

physical harm. Thus, victims who adopt pathological dissociation as the primary

defense strategy in adolescence or adulthood may be less able to engage

in self-protection when physically threatened. Dissociation has been thought

to be associated with suicide and self-injurious behaviors, and these results

confirm this association (Brodsky, Cloitre, & Dulit, 1995). Self-harm may

not be a direct response to sexual abuse but to the dissociative experiences

that result from efforts to cope with the abuse.

Results also indicate that being sexually active orbelie ving that sexual

activity is permissible can increase one’s vulnerability for physical victimization.

The incidence of self-harm in sexual abuse victims was quite dramatic.

Being sexually abused was, by far, the strongest predictor of self-harm even

when in company with other forms of child maltreatment.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – PSIHOPATOLOGIJA – SOMATIZACIJE, FIZIČNI PROBLEMI

In addition to posing psychological risks, child abuse can have serious medical consequences (Berkowitz, 2000; Wharton, Rosenberg, Sheridan, &Ryan, 2000). Many physically abused children suffer neurological or neuropsychological impairment, severe injuries, or even death (Ammerman, Cassisi, Hersen, &Van Hasselt, 1986). Teicher et al. (1997) found evidence suggestive of abnormal cortical development in nearly three quarters of their sample of sexually and physically abused children.

There are also data about such reactivity among women who have PTSD due to childhood sexual

abuse (PTSD–CSA). In a recent study, Orr, Lasko, et al. (1998) compared women

with histories of CSA who had full PTSD, partial PTSD, and no PTSD. All three

groups reacted to the trauma-related stimuli, but women with full PTSD showed

significantly greater increases in heart rate (HR) and forehead muscle tension than

did women without PTSD. In a related study, this group found that the CSAexposed

women with current PTSD or lifetime PTSD had greater HR responses

and slower habituation of skin conductance responses to auditory startle stimuli

compared to the CSA-exposedwomen without PTSD (Metzger et al., 1999). These

results suggest thatwomen with PTSD–CSA exhibit patterns of psychophysiological

responding similar to those observed in male combat veterans with PTSD when

confronted either with reminders of their childhood trauma or with startling tones.

In summary, our results with female survivors of CSA are consistent with the

findings in other PTSD subpopulations, that heightened psychophysiological reactivity

is associated with reminders of the trauma and with PTSD symptom severity.

In addition, our finding of a negative association between PTSD symptom severity

and psychophysiological reactivity for the mental arithmetic task is consistent with

other studies of PTSD. Together, these results point to the need to examine how

appraisal processes and other individual differences mediate psychophysiological

responses to laboratory stress tasks.

The physiological response to trauma-related stimuli of up to one third of

participants with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) cannot be discriminated

from that of controls. Psychophysiological measures (heart rate and blood

pressure) of 22 PTSD and 23 control civilian participants, all exposed to

missile attacks during the Gulf War, were recorded while listening to five scripts.

The physiological response of PTSD subjects with high image control (IC) was

lower than that of PTSD participants with low IC and similar to that of

non-PTSD subjects. The physiological response poorly discriminated high

IC PTSD participants from controls, but was successful in discriminating

low IC PTSD subjects from controls with 91% specificity and 92%

sensitivity. Image control is proposed as a function influencing physiological

response in PTSD.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – SPOLNOST, INCEST, SPOLNO NADLEGOVANJE, POSILSTVO

Victims of sexual assault report loss of interest and satisfaction with daily life and impairment of functioning (Ellis, Atkeson, & Calhoun, 1981). Other reactions to sexual assault include altered sleep patterns, sexual dysfunction, eating irregularities, posttraumatic symptoms, and somatic difficulties (Foa & Riggs, 1994; Nadelson, Notman, Zackson, & Gornick, 1982). Victims of sexual assault also report intense fear of revictimization and feelings of violation, vulnerability, hopelessness, loss of control, shame, anxiety, concentration difficulties, lethargy, and irritability (Becker & Kaplan, 1991; Moscarello, 1991; Nadelson et al., 1982). Sexual assault impairs a victim’s social functioning by provoking fear of strangers, social interaction, and people in close proximity, thus promoting avoidance or intense anxiety in situations similar to the sexual assault event (Steketee & Foa, 1987).

Accord ing to Rind et al. (1998), CSA gen er ally is believed

to cause severe and last ing harm to the major ity of chil dren

who expe ri ence it, and males and females are believed to be

sim i larly affected. Their anal ysis does not sup port these

beliefs for the col lege stu dent pop u la tion.

Jumper (1995) con ducted a meta-analysis of 26 stud ies of

CSA with par tic i pants from com mu nity, stu dent, and clin i cal

pop u la tions and con cluded that stu dents gen er ally emerge

from CSA expe ri ences with less psy cho log i cal adjust ment

difficulties than do CSA sur vivors in clin i cal or com mu nity

pop u la tions.

Com mu nity-Clinical Studies

In study ing women in a com mu nity set ting, Coffey,

Leitenberg, Henning, Turner, and Bennett (1996) report that

the group who reported sex ual abuse in child hood showed

greater psy cho log i cal symptomatology, includ ing higher

clin i cal lev els, than did the com par i son group, who reported

no his tory of CSA.

In another com mu nity study, Mullen, Mar tin, Ander son,

Romans, and Herbison (1995) inter viewed 298 women youn -

ger than 65 who indi cated per sonal his to ries of sex abuse in

child hood. The authors stated that fam i lies with low socio eco -

nomic sta tuses have more dis rup tion than do fam i lies with

higher socio eco nomic back grounds; there fore, abuse is

higher in these fam ilies. Mullen et al.

(1995) con cluded that child sex ual abuse is not as influ en tial

on adult psychopathology as pre vi ously thought. It is an indi -

ca tor of other types of abuse, how ever. Women who reported

more than one form of abuse gen er ally had more adult

sex ual abuse in child hood have been gen er al ized

inac cu rately. prob lems—sex ual prob lems, addic tions, low self-esteem,

eat ing dis or ders, depres sion, and psy chi at ric hos pi tal iza tion.

There are fac tors mod er at ing the abuse that were asso ci ated

with lower neg a tive out comes, such as the fam ily stay ing

intact, con fid ing with one’s mother, and hav ing a close per -

sonal friend. An exten sion of this find ing is that a child may

suf fer abuse, but close, sup port ive rela tion ships with friends

or fam ily mem bers may alle vi ate some of the long-range

prob lems asso ci ated with abuse.

Pistorello and Follette (1998) exam ined vid eo taped ses -

sions of CSA sur vivors in group ther apy to develop five cat e -

go ries of prob lems in inti mate rela tion ships reported dur ing

group ther apy. The five cat e go ries are sex-, sur vivor-, part -

ner-, rela tion ship-, and atti tude-specific state ments.

Survivors of childhood sexual abuse are more likely to experience a variety

of trauma symptoms in adulthood than adults who have not experienced

childhood sexual abuse (reviewed in Beitchman et al., 1992; Polusny &

Follette, 1995). These symptoms include depression (Pribor & Dinwiddie,

1992), suicidality (Saunders, Villeponteaux, Lipovsky, & Kilpatrick 1992),

anxiety disorders (Pribor & Dinwiddie, 1992), dissociative experiences

(Briere & Runtz, 1987), sexual problems (Davis, Petretic-Jackson, & Ting,

2001), relationship problems (Davis et al., 2001), problems with sleep

(Briere&Runtz, 1987), and borderline personality disorder (Herman, Perry,

& van der Kolk, 1989). Because only some adult survivors experience these

symptoms to a troubling degree in adulthood (Fromoth, 1986), it is possible

that etiological factors such as differences in life stress are associated with

these symptoms.

A number of studies have shown that trauma symptoms are related to the

severity of stressful life events. This has been found in research on various

kinds of traumatic events, including among victims of motor vehicle accidents

(Ehlers, Mayou, & Bryant, 1998), Vietnam veterans (Green, Grace,

Lindy, Gleser, & Leonard, 1990), Cambodian refugees (Carlson & Rosser-

Hogan, 1991), and survivors of a firestorm (Koopman, Classen, & Spiegel,

1994).

It has been noted that a history of exposure to extreme psychological stress

appears to make a person more vulnerable to experiencing psychological distress

when stressors occur later in life (Bremner, Southwick, & Charney,

1995).

Sexual abuse in childhood is a major risk factor for later sexual

revictimization (Chu, 1992; Koss & Dinero, 1989) and may lead to greater

sexual problems (Wyatt, Guthrie, & Notgrass, 1992) as well as other trauma

symptoms (Koverola, Proulx, Battle, & Hanna, 1996).

One model that can explain the vulnerability of some individuals to

trauma symptoms is “sensitization” (Post,Weiss,&Smith, 1995). The idea is

that the original trauma initiates the first episode of symptoms, but as

repeated stressful life events occur, there is a progressive sensitivity to

becoming symptomatic (McFarlane&Yehuda, 1996). Sensitivitywould also

be influenced by the severity of the life events.

Our findings provide evidence that recent life stressors and sexual

revictimization during adulthood are associated with severity of traumatic

stress symptoms in women who have been sexually abused in childhood and

who meet the DSM-IV criteria for current PTSD. Our findings provide some

evidence for the sensitization model that an initial traumatic event renders an

individual vulnerable to becoming symptomatic following subsequent

stressful life events.

Within the past 20 years, we have learned that the mental

health effects of this crime are devastating as rape survivors are the largest

group of persons with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Foa &

Rothbaum, 1998). (REBECCA CAMPBELL, SHARON M. WASCO, Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131)

…emerging research suggests that rape survivors experience

more acute and chronic physical health problems than do women who

are not victimized (Golding, 1994; Koss, Koss, & Woodruff, 1991). Sexual

assault also affects women’s sexual health risk-taking behaviors and places

some at greater risk for contracting HIV (Campbell, Sefl,&Ahrens, 2004). (REBECCA CAMPBELL, SHARON M. WASCO, Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131)

Research indicates that coping with the aftermath of

rape can cause significant stress for the family, friends, and significant others

of sexual assault survivors (Ahrens & Campbell, 2000; Burge, 1983; Remer

& Elliott, 1988).

(REBECCA CAMPBELL, SHARON M. WASCO, Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131)

Accordingly, we suggest that when a rape is committed by a known and trusted other, it is probably schema discrepant, and assimilation rather than accommodation should be the more likely outcome. Some support for this notion comes from prior work showing that women raped by known offenders, compared with those raped by strangers, were more likely to show evidence of assimilation by 3 months postrape (Mechanic, Resick, & Griffin, 1994). Assimilation should decrease the likelihood of emotional or information processing of the trauma experience, increasing the likelihood of recall deficits. Thus, we hypothesized that women raped by men they knew would be more likely to suffer from memory failure for parts of the rape compared with women raped by strangers.

More recently,

CPA has been tested as a predictor of sexual/social outcomes. For example,

Widom and Kuhns (1996) investigated the relationship between early childhood

maltreatment and subsequent promiscuity, prostitution, and teenage

pregnancy. Although CPAwas not associated with a higher risk of promiscuity

or teenage pregnancy, it was significantly associated with an increased

risk of prostitution. The Relationship Between Adult Psychological Adjustment and Childhood Sexual Abuse, Childhood Physical Abuse, and Family-of-Origin Characteristics SHANE KAMSNER MARITA P. MCCABE

Compound abuse, where a victim suffers two or more forms of abuse

either concurrently or separately, is more likely to lead to negative psychological

outcomes than any single form of childhood abuse (Bryer et al., 1987;

Fox & Gilbert, 1994; Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans, & Herbison,

1996). For example, Fox and Gilbert (1994) found that women who reported

experiencing more than one form of abuse in childhood also reported they

were significantly more depressed than those who suffered one form of abuse

in childhood. The Relationship Between Adult Psychological Adjustment and Childhood Sexual Abuse, Childhood Physical Abuse, and

Family-of-Origin Characteristics SHANE KAMSNER MARITA P. MCCABE

In an important finding, the current study indicated that a history of CPA

can contribute to a poor psychological outcome in adult life. Results showed

trauma-related symptomatology to be associated with either CPA or CSA.

This suggests that victimization through the use of violence may result in

numbed responsiveness, withdrawal, reexperience of traumatic events, and

other symptoms consonant with PTSD. Thus, psychological damage is likely

to be as severe among CPA victims as it is among CSA victims, resulting in

similar patterns of post-traumatic symptomatology for both groups. The Relationship Between Adult Psychological Adjustment and Childhood Sexual Abuse, Childhood Physical Abuse, and Family-of-Origin Characteristics SHANE KAMSNER MARITA P. MCCABE

A wide array of social, psychological, and somatic problems has been connected with childhood sexual abuse (CSA). These problems include sleep disorders, eating disorders, self-mutilation, social withdrawal, antisocial behavior, sexual dysfunction, injured sense of self, and disorders of attachment ( Bagley & Ramsay, 1985 ; Briere & Runtz, 1989 Browne & Finkelhor, 1986 ; Cohen & Mannarino, 1988; Finkelhor, 1987 ; Herman, 1981; Roth & Lebowitz, 1988; Young, 1992 ) . The consequences of undetected abuse compound the immediate trauma in child victims and are associated with grave developmental outcomes typically characterized by impaired capacities for trust, intimacy, and sexuality, and by a variety of chronic mental health problems.

(Title: Variables in Delayed Disclosure of Childhood Sexual Abuse ,  By: Eli Somer, Sharona Szwarcberg, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 0002-9432, July 1, 2001, Vol. 71, Issue 3)

Using a definition of rape that includes forced vaginal,

oral, and anal sex, the National Violence AgainstWomen Survey found that

one of six U.S. women and one of 33 U.S. men has experienced an attempted or completed rape as a child and/or adult (Tjaden&Thoennes, 1998).

(The Experience of Sexual Assault Findings From a Statewide Victim Needs Assessment, LAURA M. MONROE, LINDA M. KINNEY, MARK D. WEIST, DENISE SPRIGGS DAFEAMEKPOR, JOYCE DANTZLER, MATTHEW W. REYNOLDS, JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 7, July 2005 767-776)

Sexual

assault is associated with psychological morbidity including depression,

posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and anxiety (Ackerman, Newton,

McPherson, Jones, & Dykman, 1998; Boney-McCoy & Finkelhor, 1995;

Ellis, 1983; Roth & Lebowitz, 1988; Ullman & Filipas, 2001). Long-term

effects of child sexual abuse include suicidal behavior, personality disturbances,

substance abuse, eating disorders, and revictimization (Brier &

Runtz, 1987; Chandy, Blum, & Resnick, 1996; Dube et al., 2001; McCauley

et al., 1997; Wonderlich et al., 2001).

(The Experience of Sexual Assault Findings From a Statewide Victim Needs Assessment, LAURA M. MONROE, LINDA M. KINNEY, MARK D. WEIST, DENISE SPRIGGS DAFEAMEKPOR, JOYCE DANTZLER, MATTHEW W. REYNOLDS, JOURNAL OF

INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 7, July 2005 767-776)

Posledice

CHILDHOOD SEXUAL ABUSE is readily understood

to be a traumatic event both at time of

occurrence and for years subsequent to the

actual abuse.

Specifically,

a history of childhood sexual abuse has been

associated with consequent mood alteration such

as depression and anxiety; hindered or impaired

cognitive functioning; negative self-evaluation;

difficulty in trusting others/interpersonal relationships;

and behavioral difficulties such as

substance abuse, suicidality, self-harm, delinquent

activity and learning disabilities (American

Psychiatric Association, 1994; Beitchman,

Zucker, Hood, daCosta, & Akman, 1991; Briere

& Elliott, 1994; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986;

Gomes-Schwartz, Horowitz, & Cardarelli, 1990;

Green, 1993; Kluft, 1990; Trickett & Putnam,

1993).

Some research reports that exposure

to traumatic events such as sexual abuse in

childhood, is associated with the leading causes

of death in adulthood, including heart disease,

cancer, chronic lung disease, skeletal fractures

and liver disease (Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg,

Williamson, Spitz, Edwards, Koss, & Marks,

1998).

Literature thus far indicates that individuals

with histories of childhood sexual abuse also

report using health services at significantly

higher rates than non-sexually abused individuals

(Arnow, Hart, Scott, Dea, O’Connell, &

Taylor, 1999; Leserman, Li, Drossman, & Hu,

1998; Walker, Gelfand, Katon, Koss, Von-Korff,

Bernstein, & Russo, 1999).

Physical sequelae to childhood

sexual abuse

Different types of physical conditions appear to

be particularly common in individuals with

histories of childhood sexual abuse. Review of

the literature indicates that documented

physical sequelae to childhood sexual abuse can

be broken down into at least three categories,

including gastrointestinal and gynecological

problems, obesity and somatic complaints.

Gastrointestinal and gynecological problems

A number of studies identify a relationship

between childhood sexual abuse and the

presence of either or both gastrointestinal disorders

and gynecologic problems. Drossman,

Leserman, Nachman, Zhiming, Gluck, Toomey,

and Mitchell (1990) studied female gastroenterology

patients to identify prevalence of

sexual or physical abuse history. Patients with a

functional gastrointestinal disorder (e.g. irritable

bowel syndrome, non-ulcer dyspepsia,

chronic abdominal pain), chronic or recurrent

pelvic pain were more likely to have an abuse

history (Drossman et al., 1990; Drossman,

Talley, Leserman, Olden, & Barreiro, 1995;

Lechner, Vogel, Garcia-Shelton, Leichter, &

Steibel, 1993; Leserman, Drossman, Li, Toomey,

Nachman, & Glogau, 1996; Leserman, Li, Hu, &

Drossman, 1998; Walker, Katon, Harrop-

Griffiths, Holm, Russo, & Hickok, 1988).

This finding is corroborated by studies solely examining

abuse effects, where irritable bowel syndrome

and dyspepsia have been associated with

sexual abuse history (Longstreth & Wolde-

Tsadik, 1993; Tally, Boyce, & Jones, 1998; Talley,

Helgeson, & Zinsmeister, 1992).

Research also consistently supports a

relationship between gynecological disorders

and childhood sexual abuse. Samples from the

general population indicate a relationship

between childhood abuse (physical or sexual)

and ‘severe menstrual problems’ and urinary

tract infections (Plichta & Carmella, 1996).

Other gynecologic problems often

associated with sexual assault history include:

sexually transmitted diseases, pelvic inflammatory

disease, multiple yeast infections, premenstrual

syndrome, early hysterectomy, excessive

menstrual bleeding, genital burning, painful

intercourse, dysmenorrhea, menstrual irregularity,

lack of sexual pleasure and non-specific

gynecologic problems (Bachmann, Moeller, &

Benett, 1988; Golding, 1996).

Obesity Another line of research indicates a

relationship between childhood sexual abuse

and the physical conditions of obesity and

weight gain. This relationship is reported in

various medical patients (Felitti, 1991; Felitti et

al., 1998; Moeller, Bachmann, & Moeller, 1993;

Sansone, Sansone, & Fine, 1995); females

enrolled in weight loss programs (King, Clark, &

Pera, 1996); and in studies of both obese and

non-obese females (Wiederman, Sansone, &

Sansone, 1999). Regrettably, none of these

studies specifically sampled females with a

history of sexual abuse. While obesity may also

be secondary to events other than sexual abuse

(e.g. depression), research consistently supports

the sexual abuse–obesity relationship.

Somatic complaints Lastly, review of the

findings of physical health problems and childhood

sexual abuse would be incomplete without

mention of the more broad-based category—

‘Somatic Complaints’. ‘Somatic Complaints’ is a

categorization of physical symptoms, including

any ‘physical complaints without known biological

cause’ (Briere, 1992) as opposed to a

symptom-related syndrome or disorder as in the

case of gastrointestinal and gynecological problems.

This category is typically used to indicate

an array of physical complaints often associated

with sexual abuse yet differentiated from gastrointestinal

and gynecological disorders, including:

headaches, sleep disturbance, anorexia,

asthma, shortness of breath, chronic muscle

tension, muscle spasms and elevated blood

pressure (Briere, 1992; Felitti, 1991; Lechner et

al., 1993).

Patients with a sexual abuse history report:

greater fatigue; more headaches; increased

back, breast and face pain; increased skin and

respiratory problems; increased shortness of

breath and choking sensations; decreased

appetite; decreased sleep; less satisfaction with

their overall health status; a greater number of

actual somatic symptoms; and increased engagement

in health risk behaviors versus non-abused

patients (Leserman, Li, Drossman, & Hu, 1998;

McCauley, Kern, Kolodner, Dill, Schroeder,

DeChant, Ryden, Derogatis, & Bass, 1997;

Moeller et al., 1993; Walker et al., 1988).

Sexual abuse has also been associated with

reports of poorer overall health; greater functional

limitation; increased chronic disease;

increased medically explained and medically

unexplained complaints; greater general sleep

problems and nightmares; and more pain and

muscular tension including headaches (Bendixen,

Muus & Schei, 1994; Briere & Runtz,

1987; Golding, 1994, 1999; Golding, Cooper, &

George, 1997; Lechner et al., 1993; Springs &

Friedrich, 1992). In some cases where controlled

for, this association remained regardless of individual

characteristics and level of depression

(Golding et al., 1997).

Zdravstvo

Research with HMO enrollees suggests

that females with a history of abuse spend

significantly more money on medical healthcare

costs than females without an abuse history,

even when mental health costs were controlled.

Furthermore, females with an abuse history

have been significantly more likely to use the

emergency room for treatment than females

with a history of physical abuse or neglect

(Arnow et al., 1999; Walker, Gelfand, Katon,

Koss, Von-Korff, Bernstein, & Russo, 1999).

Among general medical, gastrointestinal and

gynecological patients, patients with a history of

childhood abuse (sexual and physical) report

higher rates of hospitalization; more days in bed

due to disability; more doctor visits; and a

greater number of lifetime surgeries than nonabused

patients (Drossman et al., 1990, 1995; Felitti, 1991;

Leserman et al., 1996; Leserman,

Li, Drossman, & Hu, 1998; Leserman, Li, Hu, &

Drossman, 1998; Moeller et al., 1993).

In addition, data from samples more specific

to sexual abuse indicate a higher incidence of

medical healthcare in abused patients. Studies of

victims of adult sexual assault indicate that

females who experience sexual assault crimes

report significantly poorer general and mental

health, endorse more physical symptoms and

report more outpatient visits than did nonassaulted

females (Golding, Stein, Siegel,

Burnam, & Sorenson, 1988; Koss, Koss, &

Woodruf, 1991).

Leserman et al.’s (1996) study found that the

severity of sexual and physical abuse history

(injury during abuse, having multiple perpetrators,

being raped) explained adult health

status. Subsequently, Leserman, Li, Drossman

and Hu (1998) found that females with more

severe sexual abuse (penetration versus other

contact) reported more: physical symptoms,

functional disability and healthcare visits than

less severely abused or non-abused females.

Similarly, Leserman, Li, Hu and Drossman

(1998) found that level of severity, as defined by

the type of sexual contact (penetration versus

other contact), was the strongest predictor of

current health status in gastrointestinal patients

with childhood sexual abuse history.

In summary, a consistent relationship has

been found between history of childhood sexual

abuse and the presence of certain adult physical

problems. It appears that females with a history

of childhood sexual abuse are more likely: to

experience increased physical health problems,

including gastrointestinal and gynecological disorders,

obesity and miscellaneous somatic complaints;

to report decreased satisfaction with

overall physical health; and to require increased

health services utilization relative to non-abused females.

Nevropsihološke posledice

Several studies of maltreated individuals have

found significant dysregulation of the hypothalamic-

pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, most

prominently increased cortisol levels, ACTHblunting

to corticotrophin releasing hormone

and flattening of the normal circadian rhythm

for cortisol (DeBellis, Baum, Birmaher, Heshaven,

Eccard, Boring, Jenkins, & Ryan, 1999;

DeBellis, Chrousos, Dorn, Burke, Helmers,

Kling, Trickett, & Putnam, 1994; Gunnar,

Morrison, Chisholm, & Schuder, 2001; Kaufman,

Birmaher, Perel, Dahl, Moreci, Nelson,

Wells, & Ryan, 1997). Heightened sympathetic

nervous system activity manifested by increased

24-hour urinary catecholamines has been noted

in two samples of traumatized children (De-

Bellis, Baum, Birmaher, Heshaven, Eccard,

Boring, Jenkins, & Ryan, 1999; DeBellis, Lefter,

Trickett, & Putnam, 1994).

There is also preliminary evidence for

immune system compromise in sexually abused

girls (De Bellis, Burke, Trickett, & Putnam,

1996).

Most recently, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)

has identified structural abnormalities in the

brains of traumatized children that correlate

with post-traumatic stress disorder and dissociative

symptoms (DeBellis, Keshavan, Clark,

Casey, Giedd, Boring, Frustaci, & Ryan, 1999).

(The Long-term Physical Health and Healthcare Utilization of Women Who Were Sexually Abused as Children, JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 7(5))

Negative environmental factors such as family conflict and stress can contribute

to the development of psychopathology in insecurely attached individuals,

whereas securely attached individuals who experience stress fail to develop

psychopathology or pedophilia (Finkelhor, 1990; Lewis, Feiring, McGuffog, &

Jaskir, 1984; Rind et al., 1998). From this perspective, an individual can develop a

vulnerability to environmental problems depending on the nature of their early

attachment experiences. Disturbances in the developmental continuity of attachment

may limit an adult’s ability to have their needs met appropriately, regulate

their emotional well-being, respond empathically to the needs of others, and seek

assistance to ameliorate abusive behaviours. The above personal characteristics

have been found to be lacking in individuals who sexually abuse children (Pithers,

Kashima, Cummings, Beal, & Buell, 1988).

Early deviant sexual experiences may serve as a template for later deviant sexual

behaviours, and inconsistent caregiving serves to undermine the security of

attachment; all this contributes to disturbances in an individual’s ability to form

intimate relationships (Hudson&Ward, 1997; Marshall, 1989). An adult who has

not developed a secure attachment system may construe early abusive sexual

experiences as enticing because they represent a form of intimacy regardless of

their deviant nature. Consequently, males who have not experienced secure

attachment may be vulnerable to placating intimacy needs by engaging in

pedophilic behaviour(Ward, Hudson,&Marshall, 1996), particularly when interpersonal

stressors such as relationship dissolution, rejection, and separation

threaten their intimacy needs (Marshall, 1989). In contrast, those who have developed

a secure attachment in childhood may be more resilient to interpersonal

trauma and intimacy problems (Masten & O’Connor, 1989). A securely attached

sexual abuse victim may not become a child sexual abuser because resilience in

the form of secure attachment may contribute to an individual’s ability to deal

with relationships and stressors.

The victims and the pedophiles came from similar, self-reported, abusive

backgrounds (see Table 3) but the results of this study indicated that the controls

and victims were both more securely attached than the pedophiles, who were

found to be insecurely attached on two constructs, namely, high on Relationships

as Secondary and lowon Confidence (lowsecure attachment). The finding for the

controls and pedophiles is in line with the research of Ward et al. (1996). Currently,

there are no published results relating to the quality of attachment for male

nonoffending victims of sexual abuse. The findings reported here provide some

support for the ASQ as a measure of attachment and as a clinical tool to measure

differences in attachment style.

These results support the notion that an insecure

attachment style may make a man vulnerable to developing pedophilic behaviours.

Conversely, those men with a secure attachment style are more resilient to

participating in pedophilic behaviour, even if they have experienced neglect

and/or abuse as children.

The current findings are consistent with the suggestion (Hudson & Ward,

1997) that insecure attachment represents a vulnerability with intimate relationships.

It isworth considering, however, that there are likely to be individual differences

between insecure styles of attachment, and these variations may not be congruent

with specific offending behavior. This has implications for treatment, as

attachment style may provide a better basis for understanding the psychological

processes associated with offenders and victims rather than categories of

psychopathology and criminality.

These findings may be underscored by the finding that both

groups experienced significant childhood victimisation, with 64% of pedophiles

and 68% of victims reporting a “severely stressful” childhood. In contrast, controls

indicated that they had not experienced childhood sexual activity with an

adult and none reported that their childhood was “severely stressful.”

Both victims and pedophiles reported that prior to the age of 14 they had

engaged “very often” in sexual activity with an adult (43% and 43%, respectively).

This finding may bring into question the contribution of sexual abuse as a

discrete factor in the development of pedophilic behavior (Finkelhor, 1986,

1990). Pedophiles reported more sexual abuse experiences with a relative (82%)

in comparison with victims (37%). This “stranger” factor is thought to mitigate

against the adverse effects of sexual abuse (Briggs & Hawkins, 1996; Finklehor,

1990). In this study, both the controls and victims were found to have secure

attachments, even though the pedophiles and the nonoffending victims had experienced

similar levels of neglect and abuse.

“children with sexual behavior problems”

Further support of the relationship between childhood

sexual abuse and SBP is found in the work of

Johnson (1988, 1989). The relationship between sexual

victimization and demonstrating SBP may be

stronger in preschool-age children than in school-age

children for boys. In a study of 47 boys with SBP, Johnson

(1988) found that 72% of the 4- to 6-year-olds had

a history of being sexually abused, whereas 42% of the

7- to 10-year-olds and 35% of the 11- and 12-year-olds

had such a history. Girls with SBP may be more likely

to have a history of child sexual abuse than boys. In a

sample of 13 female children with SBP (ages 4 to 12

years), Johnson (1989) found that 100% of the children

had a history of child sexual abuse.

The intense and wide range of problems that

these young children exhibited was quite striking. In

addition to having a mean level of SBP at the 99th percentile

on the CSBI, the children had a complex array

of other behavior and emotional symptoms and experienced

multiple stressful events, including changes

in caregivers and home placements. As hypothesized,

on the PSI, the caregivers reported significant stress

associated with raising the children and during interviews

also reported distress specifically associated with

observing and responding to the sexual behaviors.

Furthermore,

the rate of depressive symptoms was quite

remarkable, with 6 children reaching full criteria for

MDD. Symptoms of depression and anxiety in childhood

are often overlapping and interrelated. The

relationship between internalizing symptoms and

SBP remains unclear. Furthermore, assessment and

diagnosis of PTSD in children is complicated by developmental

factors, social factors, and comorbid conditions

(March, 1999). Caregivers may not be aware of

the thoughts and internal reactions young children

are experiencing and thus have difficulty responding

to standard questions about PTSD symptoms. A subgroup

of these children demonstrated verbal delays,

which would further inhibit the identification of

internal distress. In addition, the avoidance symptoms

may be reduced in many of children who were

no longer living in the homes in which they experienced

the trauma.

Furthermore,

experiencing physical abuse may increase the

likelihood of demonstrating interpersonal SBP in

young children who have been sexually abused, perhaps

by the impact on feelings of anger and shame and

beliefs about use of control with others (Hall et al.,

1998). Another potentially critical factor that was not

assessed in the current study is child neglect. Childhood

neglect has been found to be associated with significant

behavior problems, including increased risk

of sex crimes as an adult (Widom & Ames, 1994).

Poor impulse-control skills, other aggressive

behaviors, and inaccurate perceptions of social stimuli

in some children with SBP further hinder social

relationships and cause problems at school (Araji,

1997; Friedrich & Luecke, 1988; Gil & Johnson, 1993;

Horton, 1996). In addition, poor boundaries and

indiscriminate friendliness often found in young children

with SBP may place them at increased risk of

being victimized. Raising children with SBP is often

stressful for the caregiver and may lead to dysfunctional

adult-child interactions and disruptions in the

child’s residential placement. Indeed, in the present

study, caregivers reported stress associated with raising

these young children with SBP, and many of the

children had already experienced changes in their

residential placements.

In particular,

parents of asymptomatic sexually abused

children express concerns about whether their

children need to get out their feelings to prevent

future difficulties or that their children will

grow up to be molesters themselves.

Mannarino, A. P., Cohen, J. A., Smith, J. A., & Moore-

Motily, S. (1991). Six and twelve-month follow-up of

sexually abused girls. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 6,

494-511.

Animportant issue with regard to this topic is

whether existing assessment measures are adequately

detecting all of the psychological problems

that sexually abused children may exhibit.

Some examples are Friedrich’s Child

Sexual Behavior Inventory and Briere’s Trauma

Symptom Checklist for Children, which assess

abuse-related sequelae, and Mannarino and

Cohen’s Children’s Attributions and Perceptions

Scale, which measures cognitive variables

that are correlated with psychological symptomatology.

One study (Mannarino,

Cohen, Smith, & Moore-Motily, 1991) found

during a 1-year follow-up that sexually abused

girls who had been subjected to intercourse had

significantly more emotional and behavioral

symptoms than thosewhohad experienced fondling

only. This finding suggests that there may

be a sleeper effect related to the type of sexual

abuse that manifests itself over time. Unfortunately,

the length of the follow-up in this study

was relatively brief and may not have been adequate

to assess more long-term problems.

It should be noted that although the child

maltreatment field has come a long way in

terms of the development of more sophisticated

assessment measures that tap abuse-related

sequelae, we still know very little about

whether traumatic experiences cause subtle

vulnerabilities in children that do not reach the

level of symptoms but which, in combination

with other factors, may have an adverse impact

in later developmental periods.

Despite this lack of empirical data, many clinicians

provide short-term interventions for

asymptomatic sexually abused children,

including body awareness training and safety

education. In addition, some sessions with par-

192 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / April 2000

ents of asymptomatic children can be invaluable

in terms of normalizing common parental reactions

to disclosure, providing reassurance, and

discussing what potential problems parents can

be looking out for in the future.

The extensive literature on the sequelae of rape

provides a wealth of evidence that the experience of rape often leads to frank sexual

dysfunction as well as general intimacy dif. culties in the victims (e.g. Becker et al.,

1986; Feldman-Summers et al., 1979). Loss of libido, anorgasmia and sexual

aversions and phobias are common among the sexual effects. There is an extensive

literature which shows that sexual abuse in childhood can have serious negative

consequences for a person’s later sexual functioning (e.g. Courtois, 1979; Finkelhor,

1990; Jehu, 1988). Further, there is strong evidence that those who have been

sexually tortured (e.g. forced penetration with objects; infliction of pain on the

genital organs) can develop sexual dysfunction (e.g. Lunde & Ortmann, 1990).

Further, there is strong evidence that those who have been

sexually tortured (e.g. forced penetration with objects; in? iction of pain on the

genital organs) can develop sexual dysfunction (e.g. Lunde & Ortmann, 1990).

Recent research has implicated emotional abuse as a strong, possibly stronger, predictor than physical

abuse of internalizing disorders, externalizing disorders, social impairment, low self-esteem, suicidal

behavior, psychiatric diagnoses, psychiatric hospitalizations, and long-term psychological functioning

(Kaplan et al., 1999; McGee, Wolfe, & Wilson, 1997; Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans, & Herbison,

1996; Vissing, Straus, Gelles, & Harrop, 1991).

Emotional Abuse in Children: Variations in Legal Definitions and Rates Across the United States

Stephanie Hamarman, New Jersey Medical School, Kayla H. Pope, American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

Sally J. Czaja, New Jersey Medical School

CHILDMALTREATMENT,Vol. 7, No. 4, November 2002 303-311

DOI: 10.1177/107755902237261

Women in the study who had been

raped were more likely to experience posttraumatic stress disorder, major

depression, attempted suicide, and drug and alcohol problems than were

women who had not been victims of violent crimes. Other studies have

shown that women who have been raped also report more physical problems,

such as chronic pelvic pain, gastrointestinal disorders, headaches, general

pain, psychogenic seizures, and premenstrual symptoms (Koss &

Heslet, 1992). (Women’s Responses to Sexual Violence by Male Intimates

Claire Burke Draucker Phyllis Noerager Stern Western Journal of Nursing Research, 2000, 22(4), 385-406)

Survivors of childhood sexual abuse are more likely to experience a variety

of trauma symptoms in adulthood than adults who have not experienced

childhood sexual abuse (reviewed in Beitchman et al., 1992; Polusny &

Follette, 1995). These symptoms include depression (Pribor & Dinwiddie,

1992), suicidality (Saunders, Villeponteaux, Lipovsky, & Kilpatrick 1992),

anxiety disorders (Pribor & Dinwiddie, 1992), dissociative experiences

(Briere & Runtz, 1987), sexual problems (Davis, Petretic-Jackson, & Ting,

2001), relationship problems (Davis et al., 2001), problems with sleep

(Briere&Runtz, 1987), and borderline personality disorder (Herman, Perry,

& van der Kolk, 1989). (Recent Stressful Life Events,Sexual Revictimization, and Their

Relationship With Traumatic Stress Symptoms Among Women Sexually Abused in ChildhoodCATHERINE CLASSEN

RUTH NEVO,CHERYL KOOPMAN,KIRSTEN NEVILL-MANNING,CHERYL GORE-FELTON

DEBORAH S. ROSE,DAVID SPIEGEL

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 12, December 2002 1274-1290)

Approximately one in four women are raped in their adult

lifetime, which causes severe psychological distress and long-term physical health

problems. The impact of sexual assault extends far beyond rape survivors as their

family, friends, and significant others are also negatively affected. Moreover, those

who help rape victims, such as rape victim advocates, therapists, as well as sexual

assault researchers, can experience vicarious trauma. Future research and advocacy

should focus on improving the community response to rape and the prevention of

sexual assault. (Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault 20 Years of Progress and Future Directions

REBECCA CAMPBELL,SHARON M. WASCO

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131)

From that founding research on the prevalence of rape, other researchers

began to document the widespread deleterious effects that sexual assault has

on women’s lives.Within the past 20 years, we have learned that the mental

health effects of this crime are devastating as rape survivors are the largest

group of persons with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Foa &

Rothbaum, 1998). The inclusion of PTSD into the Diagnostic and Statistical

Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) in 1980was a major conceptual development

in the study of trauma associated with sexual violence. Although this

framework may be limited in its ability to capture fully the nature of sexual

assault (see Wasco, 2003), it has spawned a proliferation of research documenting

the psychological injury caused by rape. Beyond this focus on psychological

impact, emerging research suggests that rape survivors experience

more acute and chronic physical health problems than do women who

are not victimized (Golding, 1994; Koss, Koss, & Woodruff, 1991). Sexual

assault also affects women’s sexual health risk-taking behaviors and places

some at greater risk for contracting HIV (Campbell, Sefl,&Ahrens, 2004).

(Understanding Rape and Sexual Assault 20 Years of Progress and Future Directions

REBECCA CAMPBELL,SHARON M. WASCO

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 1, January 2005 127-131)

Establishing appropriate role boundaries

with sexually abused children and their families

is a challenging ethical issue for all practicing

clinicians who provide services to this population.

This can best be accomplished prior to any

clinical contact with a family. For example, a

parent may call with concerns that his or her

child has been sexually abused and about possible

associated behaviors/symptoms such as

sexually inappropriate behaviors, separation

anxiety, or sleep problems. This parent may

want to know whether the child has been

abused, how to deal with visitation and custody

issues, and also how to deal with the presenting

behavioral difficulties. (TREATING SEXUALLY ABUSED

CHILDREN AND THEIR FAMILIES

Identifying and Avoiding Professional Role Conflicts

ANTHONY P. MANNARINO,JUDITH A. COHEN

TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 2, No. 4, October 2001 331-342)

Women who have been raped experience

a range of cognitive, emotional, and behavioral symptoms. They are more

likely to meet the diagnostic criteria for a number of mental disorders than

women who have not experienced violence, including posttraumatic stress

disorder, anxiety disorders, depression, and drug and alcohol abuse (Burnam

et al., 1988; Kilpatrick et al., 1985; Winfield, George, Swartz, & Blazer,

1990). Victims of a sexual assault also are at increased risk for suicide

attempts (Bridgeland, Duane, & Stewart, 2001; Kilpatrick et al., 1985). In

addition to acute medical symptoms (Resnick, Acierno, Holmes, Dammeyer,

& Kilpatrick, 2000), victimization also can affect long-term health and sexual

functioning (Becker, Skinner, Abel, & Treacy, 1982; Ellis, Calhoun, &

Atkeson, 1980; Golding, 1994; Koss, Koss, & Woodruff, 1991). Rape can

shatter awoman’s feelings of safety and security, leaving her feeling vulnerable

and helpless (Janoff-Bulman, 1985). The woman may find herself in a

state of disequilibrium and struggle to come to terms with her victimization,

to define her experience, and to reconstruct her beliefs about the world

(Janoff-Bulman, 1985; Lebowitz & Roth, 1994). (The Role of Sexual Victimization

in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64)

Women who have been sexually victimized through force or threat of

force differ from nonvictimized women when rating women’s target behavior.

Women who have been sexually victimized perceived less sexual interest

when rating women’s target behaviors than women who have not been victimized.

The lower ratings of sexual interest for women’s behaviors may be

the result of self-protective attributions by women who have been assaulted

and have experienced victim blame following their own sexual assault. In

addition, it may be that victimized women perceive a wider range of behaviors

as not indicating sexual interest. Thus, victimized women may not

believe that their behavior is communicating an interest in sexual activities

even when others would interpret the behavior in this manner. As a result,

sexually victimizedwomen may underestimate the sexual connotativeness of

their behavior. (The Role of Sexual Victimization

in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64)

Women who have

experienced sexual aggression may be more alert for signs of sexual interest

so they can avoid unwanted sexual advances. Because sexually victimized

women report more experiences with having their sexual intent misperceived

(Abbey et al., 1996), they may be alert for potentialmisperceptions. Sexually

victimized women may view men’s behavior more sexually as a protective

measure. Women’s increased vigilance may be a way that victims assert

agency. Through increased awareness,women may feel better able to protect

themselves and to make decisions about their sexual safety. (The Role of Sexual Victimization

in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64)

The finding that women who have been victimized through force or threat

of force are more vigilant appears to contradict the finding that victimization

increases the risk of future victimization. Although women who have been

victimized appear to be more vigilant of signs of men’s sexual interest, they

may not be able to effectively detect risk at a stage early enough in the interaction

to permit escape from a sexually aggressive man. Women who have

experienced repeated victimization did respond more slowly to sexually

threatening situations in an analogue study (Wilson et al., 1999) and also may

be slower to respond in circumstances that pose a danger of revictimization.

(The Role of Sexual Victimization

in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64)

Past

research has found that, on average, victimized women have a history of a

greater number of sexual partners and sexual experiences (Himelein, Vogel,

&Wachowiak, 1994; Koss, 1985; Koss & Dinero, 1989). Generalizing these

results, we might predict that victimized women with a history of sexual

experience may be better able to accurately recognize sexual interest in a

partner than women with little sexual experience. Indeed, in this study,

women who reported sexual victimization experiences also reported a greater

number of sexual partners. This finding of increased sexual activity, however,

may be a consequence of prior victimization (Koss & Cleveland, 1997). In

addition, increased sexual activity may place a woman at greater risk for

encountering a sexually aggressive man. This finding is compatible with

other research that has found that sexually conservative women (Himelein,

1995) and women who regularly attend religious services (Mynatt&Allgeier,

1990) are less likely to be sexually victimized. In Himelein’s study, sexually

conservative women also scored higher on scales assessing adversarial sexual

beliefs and acceptance of rape myths. In addition, these women reported

fewer consensual sexual experiences and less assertiveness. Thus, it may be

that sexual conservativism reduces women’s risk through their greater wariness

and mistrust of men’s sexual motives and through their decreased

involvement in sexual activity.

(The Role of Sexual Victimization

in Women’s Perceptions of Others’ Sexual Interest,PATRICIA L. N. DONAT,BARRIE BONDURANT

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 18 No. 1, January 2003 50-64)

Depression is the symptom most commonly

reported by adult survivors of CSA (Beitchman et al., 1992; Browne

&Finkelhor, 1986; Polusny&Follette, 1995). Indeed, numerous studies find

a relationship between CSA and depression or depressive symptoms (Braver,

Bumberry, Green, & Rawson, 1992; Briere & Runtz, 1988; Hunter, 1991;

Jackson, Calhoun, Amick, Maddever, & Habif, 1990; Roland, Zelhart, &

Dubes, 1989;Yama, Tovey,&Fogas, 1993).A majority of investigations also

report a higher prevalence of major depressive disorder among sexually

abused than nonabused participants (Polusny & Follette, 1995).

CSA survivors

also report chronic and recurrent depression during adulthood

(Andrews, 1995) and longer depressive episodes in comparison to nonabused

participants (Zlotnick, Mattia,&Zimmerman, 2001). Thus, the accumulated

evidence points to depression and depressive symptoms as significant

long-term correlates of CSA.

Researchers have commonly conceptualized CSA as a major risk factor for the development of

depression and other difficulties in adulthood (Briere, 1992; Browne &

Finkelhor, 1986; Polusny & Follette, 1995).

Given these difficulties, it is not surprising that adult CSA survivors also

frequently report social isolation (Harter, Alexander, & Neimeyer, 1988),

poor social adjustment (Follette, Alexander, & Follette, 1991; Harter et al.,

1988; Jackson et al., 1990), and considerable distress and dissatisfaction in

their relationships (e.g., Briere, 1988; DiLillo& Long, 1999; Feinauer et al.,

1996; Herman, 1992; Hunter, 1991). CSA survivors report difficulties in

forming trusting, intimate relationships (Gorcey, Santiago,&McCall-Perez,

1986; Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans, &Herbison, 1994), being distant

and controlling in relationships (Whiffen et al., 2000), and avoiding the

development of close adult relationships due to fears of rejection (Alexander,

1993).

Thus, the first generation of research has shown that

CSA is a risk factor for various forms of emotional

distress, particularly depression, anxiety,

PTSD, and dissociation. Having documented

this association, researchers have moved onto a

second generation of research, the aim of which

is to understand the causal mechanisms

underlying this association; that is, why are

CSA survivors at risk for emotional distress?

Children who are sexually abused may be at

risk for feelings of shame and self-blame, especially

when the abuse was prolonged or when

the perpetrator or significant others blamed the

child for the abuse.

Individuals with a history ofCSAare likely to

abuse alcohol as adults; researchers conceptualize

alcohol use in this population as a form of

coping with the distress generated by the CSA

or as an attempt to self-medicate (Briere, 1988;

Ireland & Widom, 1994; Lindberg & Distad,

1985; Moeller, Bachmann, & Moeller, 1993).

History of childhood sexual abuse (CSA) has been linked to increased risk for long-term consequences on the lives of survivors (e.g., Adams-Tucker, 1982;

Briere & Runtz, 1988; Brooks, 1983; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Peters, 1988; Spaccarelli, 1994; Trickett & Putnam, 1993). For example, CSA survivors are more likely to experience symptoms related to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), such as moments of increased arousal and extreme affective reactivity, and alternatively, a general predisposition toward numbing of responsiveness, and emotional experiencing (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). There has been considerable focus in recent years on developing therapeutic interventions that target the cognitions and emotional experiences associated with traumatic symptoms experienced by CSA survivors. Treatments of choice typically focus on helping CSA survivors gain a greater sense of emotional stability around the traumatic episode through a variety of interventions, such as support and corrective processing of the cognitions around the event, imaginary or actual exposure (with the goal of increased habituation or decreased anxiety), and stress management, among others (e.g., Deblinger, McLeer, & Henry, 1990; Foa et al., 1999; Rothbaum, Meadows, Resick, & Foy, 2000). Although there is a growing consensus by scholars and practitioners in the identification and treatment of PTSD symptoms in cases of CSA, until lately, little attention had been given to investigating emotional coherence among this group. Specifically, there is an imperative need for further research to clarify how physiological, experiential, and expressive response domains in emotional experiencing (e.g., Ekman, 1992; Levenson, 1994) may inform psychological functioning and response

to treatment.

Womenwho are exposed to childhood sexual abuse (CSA)may experience a number of negative outcomes, some of which are evident in high rates of CSA histories in women accessing inpatient and outpatient psychiatric services (Goodman, Rosenberg, Mueser, & Drake, 1997; Mitchell, Grindel, & Laurenzano, 1996; Read, 1997). In terms of general measures of psychological disturbance, psychiatric patients who have experienced abuse are symptomatic and receive psychiatric care at an earlier age (Briere & Zaidi, 1989; Darves-Bornoz, Lemperiere, Degiovanni, & Gaillard, 1995; Goff, Brotman, Kindlon, Waites, & Amico, 1991; Read, 1998), experience more frequent/longer hospitalizations and more frequent relapses (Darves- Bornoz et al., 1995; Goff et al., 1991; Read, 1998), and are more likely to be prescribed psychotropic medications (Sansonnet-Hayden, Haley, Marriage, & Fine, 1987). In addition, childhood abuse has been specifically linked to a number of clinical problems in adulthood including suicidal ideation and attempts (Read, Agar, Barker-Collo, Davies, & Moskowitz, 2001; Resnick & Newton, 1992); eating disorders and self-mutilation (Briere&Runtz, 1989); addictive behaviors (Cameron, 1994); poor social adjustment, depression, and anxiety (Briere & Runtz, 1989); low self-esteem (Jehu, 1989); somatization (Gelinas, 1983); psychosis (Neria, Bromet, Sievers, Lavalle, & Fochtman, 2002; Read, Agar, Argyle, & Aderhold, in press; Read &Argyle, 1999; Read, Perry,Moskowitz,&Connolly, 2001; Ross, Anderson, & Clark, 1994); and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Kiser, Heston, Millsap, & Pruitt, 1991). In light of the huge range of difficulties that have been linked to CSA, it is apparent that there exist large, unexplained individual differences in the presence and presentation of mental health problems following CSA.

Outcome assessment

? Emotional states/distress

? Guilt

? Depression

? Trauma symptoms

? Interpersonal functioning

? Social introversion

? Interpersonal victimization

? Interpersonal communication

Povezanost med spolno zlorabo in PTSD

For example, given the prevalence of CSA, abuse survivors may constitute the largest single group of PTSD sufferers (Foa, Steketee, & Rothbaum, 1989). However, the proportion of sexual abuse survivors developing clinical symptoms of PTSD is estimated at only 50% (Kiser et al., 1991). In populations of sexual abuse survivors referred for psychiatric examination, estimated prevalence of PTSD rises to 73% (O’Neil & Gupta, 1991). Thus, although exposure to a traumatic stressor such as sexual abuse is by definition necessary in the etiology of PTSD, the evidence suggests that experiencing sexual abuse is not sufficient to cause PTSD symptomatology. There exist large unexplained individual differences in the presence, severity, and persistence of PTSD symptomatology following abuse.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – FIZIČNA ZLORABA

Among the various types of child maltreatment, physical abuse has been studied most frequently and is defined by the World Health Organization (1999) as “all forms of physical ill-treatment . . . resulting in actual or potential harm to the child’s health, survival, development or dignity in the context of a relationship of responsibility,

trust, or power” (p. 15). In addition to physical injuries, physical child abuse often leads to serious cognitive and socioemotional problems, including cognitive impairments and poor school performance (Eckenrode, Laird, & Doris, 1993), aggression, impulsiveness (Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1997), poor peer relations, delinquency, later substance abuse (e.g., Azar & Wolfe, 1998; Erickson & Egeland, 2002), and greater likelihood of continuing the cycle of abuse as a parent

POSLEDICE TRAVME – SMRT BLIŽNJEGA

Specifically, children and adolescents who have experienced the death of a

close relative are at greater risk for sleep problems, depressive symptoms,

isolation, school performance problems, and suicidality (Harris, 1991;

Lewinsohn, Rohde, & Seeley, 1996; Silverman & Worden, 1992; Valente,

Saunders, & Street, 1988; Weller, Weller, Fristad, & Bowes, 1991; Worden,

1996;Worden, Davies, & McCown, 1999). In addition, death of a loved one

during adolescence increases the risk of depression and suicidal behavior

during adulthood (Adams, Overholser, & Lehnert, 1994; Fikelstein, 1988;

Parker & Manicavasagar, 1986).

Familial loss research has focused primarily on the deaths of parents, with

relatively less attention being given to deaths of other close relatives and

friends. Only a few studies have examined adolescent bereavement after the death of a

close friend.

Unfortunately, the majority of bereavement

studies with adolescents have examined the effects of parental death,

with few investigating peer death. Among the studies that have examined

peer death, findings typically have been based on small community samples.

Bereavement reactions including shock, numbness, sadness,

anger, insomnia, survivor guilt, nightmares, loneliness, fear of own death,

substance abuse, suicidal ideation, and school problems have been associated

with the death of a peer during adolescence (McNeil, Silliman, & Swinhart,

1991; Sklar & Hartley, 1990; Ringler & Hayden, 2000).

POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIONALNA ZLORABA

(1992)Herman stated that the main dialectic of emotional trauma is the conflict between the need to deny unbearable experiences and the need to give testimony.

(Title: Variables in Delayed Disclosure of Childhood Sexual Abuse ,  By: Eli Somer, Sharona Szwarcberg, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 0002-9432, July 1, 2001, Vol. 71, Issue 3)

POSLEDICE TRAVME – MEDOSEBNI ODNOSI

If, as Finkelhor and Browne

(1985) suggested, early sexual trauma can profoundly “alter a child’s cognitive

and emotional orientation toward the world and cause trauma by distorting

the child’s self concept,world view, or affective capacities” (p. 531), then

there is reason to suspect thatCSAmay disrupt survivors’ long-term interpersonal

adjustment as well.

These theories suggest that one of the consequences of having been sexually

abused in childhood is the development of aberrant relationship models

that in turn lead to interpersonal difficulties.

More recently,

research has begun to examine the impact of childhood abuse within an interpersonal

context (Polusny&Follette, 1995). Although such studies typically focus on

subsequent adult social adjustment, parenting, sexual functioning, high risk sexual

behavior, and revictimization experiences, a limited number of studies (Ducharme,

Koverola & Battle, 1997; Roche, Runtz, & Hunter, 1999; Whiffen, Judd, & Aube,

1999) have investigated the partner relationships (e.g., intimacy functioning) of

childhood abuse survivors.

Interpersonal Functioning

Research suggests that survivors of childhood sexual abuse may experience

difficulties in a number of interpersonal relationship contexts. Such findings were

incorporated into recent theoretical models of the dynamics of child sexual abuse

and its long-term correlates (Briere, 1992; Cole & Putnam, 1992; Finkelhor &

Browne, 1985; Polusny&Follette, 1995;Westerlund, 1992). For example, Polusny

and Follette (1995) developed a theoretical model that highlights the function of

emotional avoidance (Hayes, 1987) in determining the long-term correlates of child

sexual abuse. This model suggests that individuals with histories of child sexual

abuse attempt to diminish negative thoughts, affective states, and memories of

abuse through various coping behaviors including dissociation, substance abuse,

casual sexual relationships, and avoidance of intimate relationships. Although

these behaviors are hypothesized to initially relieve pain by reducing or suppressing

intense emotional responses associated with abuse, their use may result in longterm

negative effects, such as feelings of social isolation, dissatisfaction with

relationships, sexual dysfunctions, and revictimization.

The few investigations that have examined the relationship between child

physical abuse and adult interpersonal relationships indicate that physically abused

children may be more aggressive (Alessandri, 1991; Kolko, 1992) or more interpersonally

sensitive than are nonabused children (Briere & Runtz, 1988; Bryer,

Nelson, Miller, & Drol, 1987; Chu & Dill, 1990).

Disturbances in

intimacy and inability to trust also arise and in turn cause, and contribute to,

relationship difficulties.

This is partly a secondary effect of frank sexual

dysfunction, and partly the result of other trauma-induced factors such as irritability,

inability to trust, fear of getting close to someone, etc. This lack of sexual interest or reduced ability to function, however, further erodes the man’s already impaired self-con. dence and sense of masculinity. Many get confused, leading to confusion in the wife, too.

Traumatic experiences can lead to various changes in the emotional relationship of the couple, with factors such as over-protectiveness, holding back, impaired trust, guilt, feelings of being let down and of letting the partner down, fears for the future, over-sensitivity and fear of rejection coming into

play (see Matsakis, 1998).

Young adults who were victims of childhood bullying have also been found to be more introverted than nonvictims (Swain, 1996), which may play a role in their heightened experience of loneliness. This loneliness is also reflected in the results found by Gilmartin (1987) in his study of “love shy” adult men (persons whose shyness with the opposite sex leaves them unable to date or marry)

Maltreatment by Parents and Peers: The Relationship Between Child Abuse,

Bully Victimization, and Psychological Distress, Renae D. Duncan, Murray State University

CHILD MALTREATMENT, Vol. 4, No. 1, February 1999 45-55

CSA is thought to have a negative impact on

interpersonal relations because it occurs in the

context of an interpersonal relationship, typically

one where a degree of safety and trust has

developed. Thus, the experience of CSA may

impede the development of trusting relationships

subsequently, particularly with romantic

partners. Furthermore, the experience of CSA

may compromise the development of a positive

sense of the self, which will inherently influence

social relationships (Cole & Putnam, 1992).

Abstract: Traumatic events can have a major impact on attachment behavior and interpersonal

relationships. In addition to the detrimental effects of post-trauma symptomatology, the traumatic

experience can become embedded in the memory structure of the individual causing a progressive

avoidance of interpersonal triggers. The traumatic experience may also have detrimental effects on

self-awareness, intimacy, sexuality and communication all of which are key elements to the

maintenance of healthy interpersonal relationships. Investigations into the effect of PTSD on

interpersonal relationships should focus on a longitudinal model of attachment. Pre-traumatic coping

mechanisms may be altered by the traumatic experience, and the relationship between pre-traumatic,

epi-traumatic and post-traumatic attachments should be addressed.

The impact of a major stressor on an individual’s relationships needs to be

considered from a longitudinal perspective. There are three windows during which

attachment patterns need to be considered when analysing the impact of disasters on

relationships: pre-traumatic, epi-traumatic and post-traumatic. Every individual has

a mode of dealing with relationships which will pre date their traumatic exposure

and is indicative of developmental experiences. Ultimately, changes that traumatic

events bring will be superimposed on these pre-traumatic ways of managing

relationships.

Particularly in those who develop post-traumatic stream disorder (PTSD) the

emerging symptoms come to have a highly detrimental effect on their personal

relationships. If the individual has developed PTSD con? ict at these times will spark

the irritability which is one of the most disruptive symptoms in terms of family

relationships. In this regard, embedded in the relationship can be a frequent

re-enactment of the fears of a recurrence of the trauma compounded by the

individual’s irritability. Paradoxically, the detrimental effects of this pattern of

reaction can be further exacerbated by the numbing and attachment disruption

which are also recognized as part of the symptomatology of PTSD. The numbing is

often experienced by individuals as a loss of a sense of empathy and as hardening.

Thus, while at one level the individual will be behaving in an increasingly agitated

and anxious way, at other times this will be mirrored by a state of apparent

detachment and affectlessness. The effects of this increasing con? ict on the family

will become reinforced because of the individual’s progressive loss of social contact

and decreasing social circle. Often these external relationships serve to mitigate

against the disruptions caused by post-traumatic consequences so that, if they are

lost, it further compounds the disruption of the homeostasis within the family.

In this way, traumatic memories have the capacity to disrupt attachments and

lead to the progressive distancing and avoidance of the interpersonal triggers that are

the stimulus for the traumatic re-enactments. If some element of the traumatic event

has involved being let down by a colleague, this can similarly evoke the involvement

of issues of trust in the traumatic memory structure. Moments of dependence and

reliance on a partner can become in. ltrated and similarly corrupted by these

experiences. Following disasters it is recognized that there is an increased incidence

of domestic violence and the abuse of children (Goenjian, 1993). This is a practical

demonstration of the capacity for these events to have signi. cant detrimental effects

on the lives of those exposed.

The rami. cations of trauma extend through the family and can have

multi-generational effects (Forman & Havas, 1990; Yehuda et al., 1998) through the

way in which trauma ruptures attachment bonds (Lifton, 1983; Allen & Bloom,

1994). Although clinical experience attests to this view, it is surprising to . nd that

few research studies have directly examined the impact of trauma on intimacy and

sexuality. Although scant, the research literature supports the notion that trauma

markedly affects relations with partners. Studies of war veterans report that they

have serious dif. culties maintaining intimate relationships and a high degree of

negative emotionality directed at spouses (Escobar et al., 1983; Carroll et al., 1985;

Jordan et al., 1992; Solomon et al., 1987; Johnson et al., 1996; Riggs et al., 1998).

Similar, . ndings are reported in studies of people who develop PTSD following

motor vehicle accidents (Blanchard et al., 1995).

POSLEDICE TRAVME – MEDOSEBNI ODNOSI - INTIMNOST

Two characteristic features of PTSD speci. cally damage intimacy. These are

avoidance and hyperarousal (criteria C and D in DSM-IV). Avoidance includes:

· markedly diminished interest or participation in signi. cant activities;

· feelings of detachment or estrangement from others;

· restricted range of affect;

· sense of a foreshortened future.

All of these features of avoidance are the results of loss of intrapsychic intimacy

and will inhibit recovery through interpersonal intimacy. Hyperarousal damages

intimacy because of increased irritability and outbursts of anger.

Intimacy

There are two broad categories of intimacy.

Intrapsychic intimacy (Sheehan, 1994) is the result of an individual achieving

adequate self-knowledge and self-acceptance which in turn foster the willingness to

share these thoughts and feelings with another. It measures the extent to which an

individual knows himself or herself and will undoubtedly in? uence the degree of

impact of a traumatic event. Resilience and vulnerability can be seen as aspects of

intrapsychic intimacy. Intrapsychic intimacy represents the capacity to develop

intimate relationships with others.

Interpersonal intimacy is seen as the result of interaction and can occur only

between people who share something meaningful with each other. It will predictably

POSLEDICE TRAVME – NAVEZANOST

Ward, Hudson in McCormack so opisali tri različne stile navezanosti in predstavili hipoteze ki vodijo do pomanjkanja intimnosti v odraslem obdobju.

Zaskrbljen stil opisujejo kot negativni pogled samega sebe in pozitiven na druge. Pri omenjeni

navezanosti bo pri posamezniku moč opaziti visoko stopnjo osamljenosti ter spolno zaskrbljenost. Pri navezanosti strahu ima posameznik negativen pogled nase in na druge. Pri njem bo prisotna želja po socialnem stiku in intimnosti kot tudi strah pred zavrnitvijo ki ga vodi v izogibanje bližini in odnosom. Zadnji stil, odpuščujoč vsebuje pozitiven pogled nase in negativen na druge. V varno navezanost lahko uvrstimo nenasilni in nespolni napad ki vključuje pozitiven poged nase in druge.

Terapevt bo v terapevtski obravnavi spodbudil klienta da sprejme odgovornost ter se osredotočil na žrtev in pri tem skušal povečati samozavest.

Early experiences with a traumatizing caregiver are well known to impact negatively the child’s attachment security, stress coping strategies, and sense of self (Crittenden and Ainsworth, 1989; Erickson, Egeland, and Pianta, 1989). (Advances in Neuropsychoanalysis, Attachment Theory, and Trauma Research: Implications for Self Psychology, ALLAN N. SCHORE, PH.D.)

With increasing interest in adult attachment, researchers began to develop and

examine various models of adult attachment styles (Bartholomew, 1990; Hazan &

Shaver, 1987; Main et al., 1985). Hazan and Shaver (1987) fashioned their model

of adult attachment in romantic relationships on Ainsworth’s childhood attachment

styles of “secure,” “avoidant,” and “ambivalent.” Main and colleagues developed

a model of adult attachment that described four categories of attachment style

including “secure-autonomous,” “dismissing,” “preoccupied,” and “unresolveddisorganized”

(Main et al., 1985).

Bartholomew has proposed a four-category model of attachment that empirically

validates Bowlby’s theory of internalworking models of self and other among

adults (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). The self model and the other model can

each be viewed as either positive or negative. Bartholomew’s attachment measure,

the Relationship Scales Questionnaire (RSQ), allows for the measurement of the

four attachment categories, as well as the two continuous underlying attachment

poles, view of self and view of other. The four attachment categories include the

secure pattern, which reflects a positive self and other model. Someone with a

secure attachment pattern has an integrated sense of self-worth and is comfortable

forming intimate relationships. The preoccupied pattern reflects a negative

self and a positive other model. A preoccupied person seeks a sense of safety by

gaining the acceptance and approval of others. The dismissing pattern reflects a

positive self and a negative other model. Someone with a dismissing attachment

style dismisses dependency needs and emphasizes independence as a means of

maintaining positive self-regard. The fearful pattern reflects a negative self and

a negative other model. Fearful individuals avoid intimacy to avoid the pain of

rejection or loss.

Attachment theorists (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991;

Bowlby, 1980) postulate that insecure attachments result from interactions that

cause individuals to doubt the trustworthiness, responsivity, and accessability of

others, and to question the integrity of the self. Similarly, PTSD comprises feelings

of distrust of others, and reflects a state of anxious apprehension that impedes

an individual’s ability to have satisfying interpersonal relationships (Stewart,

1996).

Another reason to suggest a link between attachment style and posttraumatic

stress symptomatology is that both are related to problems of affect regulation.

Alexander (1992) asserted that, in infancy, individuals develop specific affect regulation

strategies as a result of interactions with their primary caregivers. These

strategies represent the infant’s attempts to cope with anxieties arising from their

initial attachment relationships, and these coping strategies subsequently continue

into adulthood. Because PTSD can be conceptualized as a disorder of affect regulation

that results from an inability to cope with a stressful event, it is plausible that

certain attachment styles may create a vulnerability for the development of PTSD,

whereas others may act as a protective factor to guard against the development of

PTSD. As such, Alexander (1992) predicted that individuals who have a fearful

attachment style will be most at risk for the development of the more profound

disorders of affect, including PTSD.

Some empirical investigations have been conducted examining the relationship

between attachment and posttraumatic stress symptomatology (Alexander

et al., 1998; Mikulincer, Florian, &Weller, 1993). Results of these studies suggest

that individuals who possess an insecure attachment style endorse more symptoms

of posttraumatic stress than individuals with secure attachments.

REZULTATI:

This study examined the relationship between adult attachment style and

posttraumatic stress symptoms among high-risk adults who reported experiencing

childhood abuse. Results indicated that 76% of individuals in this sample had

an insecure attachment style. This finding is consistent with studies suggesting

that an abused population is more likely to have insecure attachments (Alexander

et al., 1998).

According to attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969), experiences

in close relationships can profoundly influence

perceptions of the social world. In particular, individual

differences in attachment quality have been proposed as an

important moderator of the extent to which attachment-

related information is attended to and processed (e.g.,

Fraley, Garner, & Shaver, 2000; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy,

1985). Avoidant individuals, who are uncomfortable

with closeness and intimacy, are theorized to limit the

processing of potentially distressing information, with

the goal of preventing activation of the attachment system

(e.g., Edelstein & Shaver, 2004; Fraley, Davis, &

Shaver, 1998). Anxious individuals, on the other hand,

who are preoccupied with relationship partners and

attachment-related concerns, are theorized to be hypervigilant

to information that could result in attachmentsystem

activation (e.g., Cassidy, 1994, 2000).

Findings from the few studies of attachment-related

differences in attention and memory are consistent with

these theoretical ideas, suggesting that avoidant compared

to nonavoidant individuals are less attentive to

material with emotional, attachment-related themes

(e.g., pictures depicting close relationships; Kirsh &

Cassidy, 1997; Main et al., 1985) and, perhaps as a result,

have greater difficulty recalling such material (Edelstein,

2005; Fraley et al., 2000; Mikulincer & Orbach, 1995). In

addition, although evidence is somewhat mixed, anxious

individuals appear to be particularly vigilant to

emotional, attachment-related information (e.g.,

Mikulincer, Gillath,&Shaver, 2002), which may enhance

later recall (Mikulincer & Orbach, 1995).

Individual differences in adult attachment are generally

assessed by a person’s placement on two relatively

independent continuous dimensions, avoidance and

anxiety (Fraley & Waller, 1998). Individuals with high

scores on the avoidance dimension are characterized by

chronic attempts to “deactivate” or minimize activation

of the attachment system (Cassidy, 2000; Edelstein &

Shaver, 2004): In stressful situations, avoidant individuals

tend to minimize expressions of distress (Fraley &

Shaver, 1997) and are unlikely to turn to or provide support

for others (e.g., Edelstein et al., 2004; Fraley &

Shaver, 1998; Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992). They

dislike physical and emotional intimacy (Brennan,

Clark, & Shaver, 1998; Fraley et al., 1998) and grieve less

following a breakup compared to nonavoidant adults

(Fraley & Shaver, 1999).

Attachment anxiety, in contrast, appears to reflect

“hyperactivation” of the attachment system (Cassidy,

2000): Individuals scoring high on the anxiety dimension

report fears of being alone and are preoccupied

with intimacy and relationship partners. They are hypervigilant

to attachment figures and attachment-related

concerns (e.g., Mikulincer, Birnbaum, Woddis, &

Nachmias, 2000; Mikulincer et al., 2002) and are easily

distressed by even brief separations from attachment figures

(Feeney & Noller, 1992; Fraley & Shaver, 1998). In

this two-dimensional framework, individuals who score

low on both dimensions are considered secure.

Although any kind of unwanted sexual experience is

likely to be emotional and possibly traumatic, the most

severe cases are presumably experienced most negatively

and, due to their potentially threatening nature,

should be most likely to activate attachment-related concerns

and defenses (e.g., Mikulincer, Florian, & Weller,

1993).

Bowlby (1969, 1973, 1980, 1988) proposed that the quality of attachment

between an infant and caregiver formed a template for the development of

relationships in later life. Specifically, he proposed that secure early attachment

to caregivers, associated with an attuned responsive parenting style,

leads in later life to positive peer relationships, fulfilling romantic relationships,

and productive parental relationships. In contrast, insecure or disorganized

early attachment, associated with unresponsive, neglectful, or abusive

parenting, leads in adulthood to problematic, neglectful, abusive, or violent

romantic and parental relationships, and to problematic relationships with

peers.

Abusive experiences during childhood are thought to disrupt the attachment

process. As a result, the interpersonal schemas that those with a history

of abuse bring with them to adulthood tend to be negative and unwavering

across different relationships. Such schemas (e.g., abuse is a way of connecting

with another person) may motivate behavior that increases the likelihood

of subsequent victimization (Cloitre, Cohen, & Scarvalone, 2002).

Revictimization, including intimate partner violence (IPV), is well documented

among survivors of childhood abuse (Rodriguez et al., 1998; Schaaf

& McCanne, 1998). In some early work, Briere and Runtz (1987) found that

women who were sexually abused were more likely than their counterparts

who were nonabused to be victims of physical abuse by a partner. Coid et al.

(2001) found two to three times the risk of domestic violence among women

who reported CPA or CSA. Whitfield, Anda, Dube and Felitti (2003) found

approximately three times the likelihood of current IPV in women who

reported CPA and approximately two times the likelihood in women who

reported CSA or witnessed their mother being battered. Risk of victimization

increased with the number of types of childhood maltreatment. This study

was limited, however, because the measure of IPV consisted of a single

screening question.

Another way of looking at interpersonal difficulties

is through the lens of attachment theory,

which proposes that early relationships between

children and their caregivers shape the

development of children’s internal working

models of the self and others. Working models

are cognitive schemas that reflect a child’s sense

of self-worth and his or her expectations about

the emotional responsiveness of significant others.

For instance, children who experience

warmth and consistency in their relations with

their caregivers will develop a working model

of the self as lovable and a working model of

others as loving and reliable. Children who are

sexually exploited may develop negative working

models of both the self and others. Specifically,

these children may form a working model

of the self as shameful and a working model of

others as untrustworthy, unresponsive to their

emotional needs, and abusive.

Navezanost (vrste, kako se kaže)

Bowlby (1969, 1973, 1980) pointed to the important

role of parent-child bonds for the

healthful development of the child. He observed

that when parent-child relationships

were poor, children suffered. Such children often

become anxious, angry, or depressed. Subsequent

research has demonstrated quite clearly

that all manner of problems result from inade-quate parenting (Bretherton, 1985; Kolvin,

Miller, Fletting, & Kolvin, 1988; Loeber, 1990;

Paterson & Moran, 1988).

Ainswoth and her colleagues (Aisworth,

Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) distinguished

three types of parent-child attachments: secure,

anxious/ambivalent, and avoidant.Whenthese

early attachment bonds are secure, the child develops

the self-confidence and skills necessary

to form effective relationships with children

outside the family.

Bowlby (1969) suggested

that parent-child relationships provided the developing

child with a template for all future relationships.

In secure bonds, the child learns that

he or she has the qualities to be loved and that

other people can be loving. This gives the child

not only self-confidence but also confidence in

others. These secure bonds instill a resiliency in

the child such that he or she can cope adequately

with the ups and downs of life.

Insecure parent-child attachments, on the

other hand, typically fail to produce these features

of resilience. Where parents offer little or

no support to their children and are inconsistent

in their responses, they are said to form anxious

ambivalent bonds with their children. Children

who are products of this type of relationship

have negative views of themselves and are desperate

to be close to others, but at the same time,

they are afraid of closeness for fear they will be

rejected. When parents are cold and distant and

lack emotional expressiveness, children will develop

an avoidant style. Avoidant children see

others as untrustworthy and will attempt to

maintain a safe distance from other people to

protect themselves. Both anxious/ambivalent

and avoidant children are likely to have low selfesteem,

poor relationship skills, and be unable

to handle life’s daily problems. These children

are vulnerable to stress because they have no

confidence in their capacity to deal with problems

and because they feel they have no one to

rely on whom they can trust. This incapacity to

deal with problems leads to the development of

an inadequate coping style by which the child,

and later the adult, either avoids facing difficulties

or simply responds by being emotionally

overwrought and withdrawn. Both these inadequate

coping styles lead to self-indulgence as a

way to deal with problems. In addition, these

children will be readily responsive to the attention

of others, even though they are wary, if not

afraid, of closeness.

(THE ORIGINS OF SEXUAL OFFENDING, WILLIAM L. MARSHALL, LIAM E. MARSHALL, TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 3, July 2000 250-263)

POSLEDICE TRAVME – OSEBNOST, SAMOPODOBA, VIDENJE DRUGIH

If, as Finkelhor and Browne

(1985) suggested, early sexual trauma can profoundly “alter a child’s cognitive

and emotional orientation toward the world and cause trauma by distorting

the child’s self concept,world view, or affective capacities” (p. 531), then

there is reason to suspect thatCSAmay disrupt survivors’ long-term interpersonal

adjustment as well.

Abused children do not receive protection when they need it. As noted by Herman (1997), "At the moment of trauma the victim is utterly helpless. Unable to defend herself, she cries for help, but no one comes to her aid. She feels totally abandoned. The memory of this experience pervades all subsequent relationships" (p. 137). For child abuse survivors, problems with protection persist into adulthood. They often have difficulty setting limits in interpersonal relationships, defending themselves in conflict situations, and guarding against repeated victimizations (Briere, 1992). In addition, many have trouble protecting their own children from abuse (Goodwin, McCarthy, &DiVasto, 1981; McCloskey &Bailey, 2000; Oates, Tebbutt, Swanston, Lynch, &O'Toole, 1998; Spieker, Bensley, McMahon, Fung, &Ossiander, 1996).

Many trauma researchers have recognized a connection between receiving protection in childhood and feeling protected in later life. For example, according to van der Kolk, van der Hart, and Marmar (1996).

Theories of the long-term impact of child abuse typically stress concepts such as affect regulation, identity, cognitive schemas, and interpersonal trust (e. g. , Alexander, 1992; Briere, 1996; Cole &Putnam, 1992; Horowitz, 1997; Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Linehan, 1993; van der Kolk &Fisler, 1994). I know of no theories of child abuse that emphasize the development of self-protection.

This article presents a case for interpersonal protection as an organizing construct in abuse research and treatment.

Sexual assault upsets perceptions of safety and may significantly affect a victim’s future ability to feel safe.

Marital sexual assault poses a greater threat to a victim’s sense of security and safety in the world than do other forms of sexual assault because it violates one’s safety expectations (Foa & Riggs,1994) and may reduce one’s confidence to judge others and to form safe relationships (Finkelhor&Yllö, 1982; Goodman, Koss,& Russo, 1993; Kilpatrick, Best, Saunders,&Veronen, 1988; Whatley, 1993).

Lachmann and Beebe (1997) point out that an event becomes traumatic when it ruptures the individual’s selfobject tie, without opportunity for repair, thereby dramatically altering her self-state.

With trauma, personal responsibility is distorted due to the lack of control one feels

when the trauma is happening. This confronts the patient with the disparity between what he wanted to happen and the tragic events that actually occurred.

In the comprehensive review carried out

by Kendall-Tackett et al. (1993), the authors conclude that

the impact of sexual abuse upon children does not necessarily

yield a distinct identifiable syndrome. Instead, the experience

manifests itself in a variety of symptomatic and pathological

behaviours. These problems may not remit but may result in

lifelong impairments that include fear, depression, substance

abuse, dissociative disorders and sexual dysfunction (Browne

and Finkelhor, 1986). Furthermore, it has been suggested that

childhood abuse often disrupts children’s development by

stimulating primitive coping strategies and by creating cognitive

distortions of self, others and the future (Briere, 1992).

The

self-loathing that many traumatized women experience

may generalize into thinking of both the self and the

body as bad and ugly (Hyman, 1999; Miller, 1994). The

body, the site of the original abuse, was violated, and

this abuse included both emotional and physical

boundary violations. These boundary violations may

lead to self-harm, which temporarily helps define the

body boundaries.

Self-image and self-esteem also appear to be key factors in some.

The overall negative self-image thus caused can lead to predictions of failure in all

matters, including sex, and to a sense that one is not attractive even when there is

no physical dis. gurement.

PROCESI, KI RAZLAGAJO POSLEDICE

–SPOMIN IN OBLIKOVANJE SHEM O SEBI, SVETU

vpliv travme na razvoj shem, motnje spomina, pomankljiva integracija izkušenj, manjkajoč občutek konstantnosti

When considering the psychological impact of trauma, much has been written about possible effects on memory and self representation. Clinical case studies and theoretical literature describe disturbances such as amnesia for early autobiographical memories, instability of self image, disturbed cognitions relating to self; lack of a sense of a self; identity confusion; and poor or negative internal self-representations (Briere 1989, 1992; Herman and van der Kolk, 1987; McCann and Pearlman, 1990; Parkin, 1987; Putnam, 1990; Reviere, 1996; Schetky, 1990; Schultz, 1990; van der Kolk, 1987; Zelikovsky and Lynn, 1994). In general, the presence of trauma in childhood is presumed to create a disruption in the continuity and stability of experience theoretically necessary for normative development of schematic representations of self and the world, at least in part, through autobiographical memories (McCann and Pearlman, 1990; Stern, 1985). More specifically, the sustained attention that traumatized children devote to physical and/or emotional survival is thought to interfere (through any of various mechanisms in the memory processes) with encoding or retention of early autobiographical memories. Such a narrowing of attention has been theorized to result in distortions in memory and thus, impairment in development of a coherent, well developed self schema.

It has been posited that the memory impairment often observed in trauma may be linked to an underlying motivation to preserve the integrity of nascent schemas and psychological well-being in a child. Specifically, the inclusion of trauma in an autobiographical narrative may threaten basic schematic structures and assumptions (e.g. of safety); thus, one adaptation may be to leave the traumatic material unassimilated, thereby disrupting memory and basic schema formation (Bartlett, 1932; Fine, 1990; Horowitz, 1991; Janoff-Bulman, 1989; Piaget, 1967; Schachtel, 1959; Singer and Salovey, 1991). Further, if trauma is encountered during child hood, at a time when schemas and beliefs about the self and world are forming, traumatic experience may interfere with development of supraordinate, general schemas that create a sense of continuity in self, memory, and meaning (Fine, 1990; Horowitz, 1991). For a child who has not yet achieved the internal stability afforded by adequate schematic development, such disruption could have significant affects. The attempt to resolve this disruption may result in significant distortions in self-experience as the child attempts to organize experience and self in a way that allows some degree of perceived control. Since schemas are likely not available for assimilation of traumatic information, and accommodation of developing schemas to include traumatic material precludes a basic sense of mastery of self and eorld, dissociation of traumatic events and resulting memories may provide for the preservation safety needed by traumatized child (Horowitz, 1991; van der Kolk et al., 1989). As such, the consolidation of memories related to self may be disrupted. This may lead to a self schema that remains unintegrated with subsequent experience, or the trauma can become an organizing frame frame for potentially impaired self development (Barclay, 1986; Barclay and DeCooke, 1988; Barsalou, 1988; Brewer, 1986; Bruhn, 1990; Schachter et al., 1989).

If large segments of autobiographical memory are separated from personal identity, in part due to schematic disruptions, the usual frame of reference for continual calibration and definition of self is lost. Thud trough the a process of mutual influence, the disruption of consistency in life experience may create a propensity for disruption in development of schemas and a disruption in a development of a coherent autobiographical memory system (Barclay, 1986, 1988; Bartlett, 1932; Bruhn, 1990; Neisser, 1988).

POSLEDICE TRAVME – SOOČANJE S KASNEJŠIM STRESOM

It has been noted that a history of exposure to extreme psychological stress

appears to make a person more vulnerable to experiencing psychological distress

when stressors occur later in life (Bremner, Southwick, & Charney,

1995). This was demonstrated in a study of female and male Vietnam veterans

that found that stressful life events occurring after the war were associated

with greater PTSD (King, King, Fairbank, Keane, & Adams, 1998).

Among Holocaust survivors, both cumulative and recent stressful life events

were significantly related to PTSD (Yehuda et al., 1995). Similarly, among

Israeli soldiers who suffered combat stress reactions during the 1982 Lebanon

War, additional negative life events were found to be associated with

greater PTSD and recovery from PTSD (Solomon, Mikulincer, & Flum,

1989). Furthermore, among survivors of a firestorm, stressful life events

occurring prior to, during, and after the firestorm were each found to have

independent relationships in predicting later PTSD symptoms (Koopman

et al., 1994). (Recent Stressful Life Events,Sexual Revictimization, and Their

Relationship With Traumatic Stress Symptoms Among Women Sexually Abused in ChildhoodCATHERINE CLASSEN

RUTH NEVO,CHERYL KOOPMAN,KIRSTEN NEVILL-MANNING,CHERYL GORE-FELTON

DEBORAH S. ROSE,DAVID SPIEGEL

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 17 No. 12, December 2002 1274-1290)

POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIJE IN AFEKTI

Attempts to suppress negative affect associated with the trauma

may lead to generalized constricted affect, an inability to have tender or loving

feelings, and problems with making and maintaining relationships. Drug

and alcohol abuse may also occur as a way of avoiding trauma-related affect.

With hyperarousal, a variety of difficulties may occur including an exaggerated

startle response, hypervigilance, poor concentration, irritability or outbursts

of anger, and difficulty falling or staying asleep.

Being alone invites the overwhelming pain

and affect associated with the trauma and self-badness

to be experienced. Abused women, many of whom

have been victims of male perpetrators as children,

report fear of being alone (Lobel, 1992). The fear of

being alone is related to the isolation in which the

abuse took place—no one saw or heard and certainly

no one stopped the assault. Lobel found that 63% of

women who had been sexually abused as children

reported feelings of self-hatred, worthlessness, and

guilt when they were alone.

That is, numbing may involve suppression of the expression

of emotions (at least some components of emotional

responding), which would lead to an increase in

emotional experiencing (in other components). Since theories

propose numbing to occur in response to trauma

cues (e.g., Foa, Zinbarg, & Rothbaum, 1992; Litz, 1992),

emotional suppression and incongruities in emotional experiencing would

be most likely to occur subsequent to exposure

to trauma cues. This pattern may therefore account

for the difficulties in processing of traumatic experiences

in individuals with PTSD as well as the maintenance of

PTSD symptomatology.

The theories proposed to account for the change in

self-report of emotional experiencing and increased physiological

arousal that accompany emotional inexpressivity

and suppression emphasize the physical and psychological

effort required to inhibit emotions (e.g, Cacioppo et al.,

1992; Notarius & Levenson, 1979).

Trauma leaves in its

wake a loss of trust, of faith, of safety, of connection. It is often a

frightening confrontation with helplessness, with the fear and rage that

helplessness induces, and with a numbness that mutes these painful

states of mind. It is perhaps for these reasons that successful treatment

of people who have been traumatized often uses multiple modalities,

occurs in stages, and extends over a lengthy period of time (1).

(THE TRAUMA OF PROFOUND CHILDHOOD LOSS: A PERSONAL AND PROFESSIONAL PERSPECTIVE

Francine Cournos, M.D., Psychiatric Quarterly, Vol. 73, No. 2, Summer 2002)

Symptoms of emotional numbing are a core feature

of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in the nomenclature

of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of

Mental Disorders (4th ed. [DSM–IV]; American Psychiatric

Association, 1994). In DSM–IV, emotional

numbing is assessed in symptom cluster C. Criteria for

cluster C include symptoms of avoidance and emotional

numbing, whereas cluster B assesses symptoms

of re-experiencing and cluster D symptoms of hyperarousal.

Emotional numbing involves diminished interest

in activities, feelings of detachment from others,

and the restriction of affect (Foa, Davidson,&Frances,

1999). Such features of emotional numbing have salient

clinical impact, as they are likely to produce personal

distress and impairment in interpersonal functioning

(Carrion & Steiner, 2000). Moreover, the

symptoms of emotional numbing are closely associated

with features of major depression and dysthymia.

A Prospective Test of the Association Between Hyperarousal and Emotional Numbing in Youth With a History of Traumatic Stress - Carl F. Weems, Kasey M. Saltzman, Allan L. Reiss, and Victor G. Carrion - Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 2003, Vol. 32, No. 1, 166–171

Research employing adult samples has shown that

symptoms of emotional numbing are strongly associated

with symptoms of hyperarousal (e.g., Flack, Litz,

Hsieh, Kaloupek,&Keane, 2000; Litz et al., 1997), and

theorists (Litz, 1992; Litz et al., 1997) have hypothesized

that emotional numbing may result from emotional

exhaustion produced by prolonged periods of

arousal. More specifically, symptoms of emotional

numbing are thought to be caused by the depletion of

cognitive and emotional resources due to prolonged

hyperarousal. Support for this theory comes from a

variety of sources, including animal models of inescapable

shock and humans exposed to traumatic events (see

Flack et al., 2000; Litz, 1992; Litz et al., 1997; Van der

Kolk, Boyd, Krystal, & Greenberg, 1984).

A Prospective Test of the Association Between Hyperarousal and Emotional Numbing in Youth With a History of Traumatic Stress - Carl F. Weems, Kasey M. Saltzman, Allan L. Reiss, and Victor G. Carrion - Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 2003, Vol. 32, No. 1, 166–171

Results in this sample provided support for the theory

that emotional numbing may develop as a result of

chronic hyperarousal in youth.

The

prospective design used in this study added to an understanding

of the association between hyperarousal

and emotional numbing by showing that emotional

numbing was not a robust prospective predictor of

hyperarousal. Such findings suggest the direction of

the association. That is, hyperarousal is associated

with later emotional numbing, but emotional numbing

does not robustly predict later hyperarousal. Taken together,

these results provide preliminary support for

the theory that emotional numbing may result from

emotional exhaustion or the depletion of cognitive and

emotional resources due to prolonged hyperarousal in

youth.

A Prospective Test of the Association Between Hyperarousal and Emotional Numbing in Youth With a History of Traumatic Stress - Carl F. Weems, Kasey M. Saltzman, Allan L. Reiss, and Victor G. Carrion - Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 2003, Vol. 32, No. 1, 166–171

POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIJE IN AFEKTI – KRIVDA IN SRAM

Self-blame following sexual assault has been studied extensively, particularly

in relation to Janoff-Bulman’s theory (1979). In general, this research

has found that many victims of sexual assault use both characterological selfblame

and behavioral self-blame; however, both types of attributions are

associated with higher distress both immediately postrape and over time

(Arata, 1994; Frazier, 1990; Frazier&Schauben, 1994; Hill&Zautra, 1989;

Katz & Burt, 1988; Mandoki & Burkhart, 1989). Furthermore, engaging in

self-blame is associated with greater use of maladaptive coping strategies

(Arata & Burkhart, 1998; Arata, 1994) and higher rates of posttraumatic

stress disorder (Arata & Burkhart, 1996).

The research on self-blame following adult sexual assault has focused primarily

on the distinction between behavioral and characterological selfblame

and, more recently, whether there are differences in feelings of blame

versus responsibility or avoidability (Abbey, 1987).

Women with a history of child sexual abuse were more likely to

engage in self-blaming attributions regarding the rape. That is, they were

more likely to blame themselves for the rape, including seeing themselves as

having deserved the rape, being a victim type, being a bad person, or not

being able to take care of one’s self. At the same time, women with a history

of child sexual abuse also reported more societal blame. They tended to feel

that theworld is unsafe forwomen, that there are many emotionally disturbed

individuals around, and that no one is ever available to help when it is needed.

The fact that thesewomen also

report greater use of cognitive strategies is more difficult to interpret. Presumably,

cognitive strategies are more adaptive, however, this finding is consistent

with Burt and Katz’s (1987) description of rape victims alternating

between emotion-focused (such as nervous) coping and problem-focused

(such as cognitive) coping, with use of all coping strategies decreasing as

symptoms decrease. Additionally, the results suggest that women may continue

to use the same maladaptive coping strategies for dealing with adult

assault as were employed for their childhood abuse. Coffey et al. (1996b) and

Leitenberg et al. (1992) found that disengagement or avoidant coping strategies

were the types of coping most frequently employed bywomen abused as

children. One issue to consider is whether these coping strategies might also

serve as vulnerability factors to revictimization.

Shame is a negative and disturbing emotional experience involving feelings of self-condemnation and the desire to hide the damaged self from others (Lewis, 1992; Tangney, 1995). It is a state in which the whole self feels defective, often as a result of a perceived failure to meet social and self-imposed standards. Clinical, theoretical, and empirical work have emphasized shame as a common consequence of child sexual abuse (Feiring, Taska, & Lewis, 1996, 2002; Finkelhor & Browne, 1987; Nathanson, 1989).

Previous results from this study showed that shame for the abuse is common at the time of discovery and generally decreases during 1 year’s time (Feiring et al., 2002).

It is a self-conscious emotion that requires the cognitive ability to have a sense of self and evaluate one’s behavior against a standard (Lewis, 2000). Shame, as measured by body posture and facial expression, can be observed in children as young as 3 years of age (Lewis, Alessandri, & Sullivan, 1992). It is not until later, around the age of 8 years, that children are able to generate appropriate examples of shame experiences (Ferguson & Stegge,1995).

Guilt, like shame, is a self-conscious emotion. Both emotions focus on the self and involve negative feelings. However, guilt focuses on specific aspects of the self that are perceived as causing failure, whereas shame focuses on the whole self. Guilt concerns one’s actions. Shame concerns one’s entire being. Guilt motivates taking action to repair the perceived failure (e.g., being on time rather than late for dinner dates with a close friend), whereas shame motivates hiding the self from exposure and inaction (Tangney & Dearing, 2002).

A consistent finding in shame research is that this self-conscious emotion often motivates an avoidance response such that the individual wants to hide the exposed self (Barrett, Zahn-Waxler, & Cole, 1993; Tangney, 1995). Strong negative emotions associated with traumatic events are aversive. They promote cognitive and behavioral avoidance, which, in turn, prolong PTSD symptoms (Berliner & Wheeler, 1987; Foa &Riggs, 1994). Earlier abuse-related shame was expected to show longterm consequences for the experience of PTSD symptoms. Individuals with high abuse-related shame during the 1st year following abuse discovery were expected to be most at risk for experiencing clinically significant levels of PTSD symptoms 6 years following abuse discovery.

Findings for individual patterns of persistence in shame across time showed that high levels of shame are likely to abate. This suggests that although abuse-related shame was common at abuse discovery, there was a good chance that such high levels of shame would not become characteristic of the individual across several years. For individuals low in shame at abuse discovery and for those low in shame 1 year later, the prognosis was very good.

Feedback from significant others about one’s failure to be good and lovable should be a primary contributor to shame and its persistence across time.

Higher levels of shame in children are associated with parental hostility, rejection, negative affective displays and comments during learning, and minimal recognition for good behavior (Alessandri & Lewis, 1996; Ferguson & Stegge, 1995; Stuewig & McCloskey, 2005). It has been suggested that the more the child hears and internalizes deficiency messages that he or she is not and never will be good enough or lovable, the greater the likelihood of shame (Potter-Efron, 1989).

It is not just fear that motivates avoidance of thinking about traumatic events. Our findings suggest that to cognitively and emotionally process traumatic experiences, therapists must help clients to confront both shame and fear. Although clients may not spontaneously volunteer feelings of shame, shame can readily be observed in a client’s use of language, reluctance to disclose, and nonverbal behavior (e.g., avoidance of eye contact, covering the face, head down, body collapsed, body hidden with a pillow or coat).

(The Persistence of Shame Following Sexual Abuse: A Longitudinal Look at Risk and Recovery) - vsi odstavki

ABSTRACT:Guilt about surviving a traumatic event is thought to be an associated feature of posttraumatic stress

disorder (PTSD). Shame is an emotion closely related to guilt but is a distinct affective state. Little is

known regarding the role of shame in PTSD and there are no studies of PTSD where shame and guilt

are examined simultaneously. We used a measure of shame- and guilt-proneness in 107 community

residing former prisoner of war veterans all of whom had been exposed to trauma. The measure of

shame-proneness was positively correlated with PTSD symptom severity whereas guilt-proneness

was not. This study provides the first empirical data regarding a possible role for shame in PTSD and

may have important therapeutic and theoretical implications.

Many combat veterans experience profound feelings

of guilt following the survival of a trauma and guilt can

be related to painful wartime memories (Glover, 1984;

Kubany, 1994).

Although guilt and shame are

terms that are often used interchangeably, current theoretical

and empirical literature underscores that these are distinct

affective experiences (Lewis, 1971; Lindsay-Hartz,

1984; Tangney, 1990, 1991). Lewis (1971) defined guilt

as the self’s negative evaluation of particular behaviors

while shame involves the self’s negative evaluation of the

entire self.

In contrast, shame is conceived of as a more devastating

and painful emotion in which the entire self, not just the

behavior, is negatively evaluated (Tangney, 1991). Shame

theoretically involves painful self-scrutiny, and feelings of

worthlessness and powerlessness (Lindsay-Hartz, 1984;

Tangney, 1990). Also hypothesized is an associated sense

of sudden and unexpected exposure, which renders the

individual feeling diminished or defective (Lewis, 1971).

Shame conceptually, therefore, may lead to a global and

debilitating painful affective reaction with a desire to hide

or escape from others (Gramzow & Tangney, 1992).

Stone (1992) hypothesized

that traumatized individuals with PTSD suffer from

symptoms of both guilt and shame. In a largely conceptual

article, he points out that in combat an example of

guilt is the troubling feeling that one survived when others

did not, whereas shame is the feeling of doubting the

right to exist. Theoretically, therefore, guilt may be related

to actions performed and shame to perception of oneself

(Janoff-Bulman, 1979).Wong and Cook (1992) conducted

the only empirical study on shame in those with PTSD.

Veterans with PTSD scored higher on measures of shame

than veterans with substance abuse or depression. Neither

trauma exposure nor severity of PTSD symptoms was ascertained

and, therefore, limited conclusions can be drawn

from this study.

REZULTATI:

Contrary to our initial hypothesis, only shameproneness

and not guilt-proneness, as measured by the

TOSCA, correlated positively with severity of PTSD

symptoms. Those withPTSDhad higher shame-proneness

scores than those without.

Self-blame as a consequence

of sexual abuse has received a great deal of attention

in the literature, although much of this research has

focused on self-blame following adult sexual assault. In studies of women with histories of child sexual

abuse, self-blame and self-denigratory beliefs have

been found as frequent effects (Courtois, 1988; Herman,

1992; Jehu, 1988).

Females who blame themselves for their

child victimization may have feeling of guilt, shame,

and low self-worth with subsequent increases in sexual

behavior because the victim perceives herself as only

being worthy of relationships if sex is offered.

Cutting, unlike childhood abuse, is within the control

of the trauma survivor. Of course, the relief it brings is

short-lived and often leads to shame and guilt, and the

cycle of pain, relief, and shame starts again.

The negative reactions caused by a traumatic

even often lead to shame, self-loathing and self-blame, even when the individual had

no responsibility for the event.

Feelings of guilt also seem to be a relevant factor. Guilt about the accident

itself, however irrational, is common. There is also guilt, commonly felt by these

trauma victims, about letting down one’s family or partner, being a burden on the

partner or family, and being ‘worthless’.

Children who are sexually abused may be at

risk for feelings of shame and self-blame, especially

when the abuse was prolonged or when

the perpetrator or significant others blamed the

child for the abuse.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIJE IN AFEKTI – SRAM IN GNUS

V raziskavi so Dumbn in Marshall ter Langton predvidevali, da izkušnja sramu ki je posledica spolnega napada v otroškem obdobju v otroku poveča osebno stisko, občutje krivde, poveča kognitivne motnje s tem pa poraste tveganje ponovne obnovitve.

Krivda in sram sta opisana kot samega sebe zavedajoče občutje. Skrivanje pred drugimi je dejanje, ki je bilo v empiričnih raziskavah opisano s strani Barlowa in drugih zopet drugi pa so sram opisali kot pozunanjenost graje. ž

Priznavanje oziroma izpoved sta Mascolo in Fischer opisala kot krivdo in pripravljenost obnoviti prvotno stanje ki vključuje opravičilo. Pri krivdi je v ospredju dejanje samo medtem ko je pri sramu na prvem mestu oseba. Zato je razumljivo, da ob krivdi pride do izraza empatija do misli in dejanj bolj kot do osebe. Razkritje samega sebe je mejnik v katerem razlikujemo med krivdo in sramom. Izkušnja sramu vključuje obsojanje samega sebe kot nesposobnega ali slabega ali da razmišlja o sebi na način kako ga drugi obsojajo.

Zunanji sram odseva skrb kako je posameznik viden s strani drugega. Od slednjega je tudi odvisno kako pomembna je druga oseba v našem življenju.

SHAME AND GUILT

Shame and guilt are often cited as two different but

related moral emotions that regulate social behavior.

Although the terms are often used interchangeably,

there are important conceptual differences

(Tangney, 1990, 1991; Tangney, Wagner, &

Gramzow, 1992). Both shame and guilt are “negative”

or uncomfortable emotions and as such are usually

correlated. Both also deal with self-evaluative judgments,

in that we judge ourselves, and our actions,

according to internal standards. H. B. Lewis (1971)

theorized, however, that the key difference between

shame and guilt concerns the distinction between

“the self” and “behavior.” Shame focuses less on specific

behaviors and more on the evaluation of the

entire self against internalized standards. Guilt, on

the other hand, reflects feelings about actions that are

inconsistent with internalized standards. The two

emotions have been shown to lead to different “action

tendencies” (Lindsay-Hartz, 1984; Tangney, Miller,

Flicker, & Barlow, 1996). When guilt is experienced,

people are motivated to make reparations for the

behavior. When shame is felt, people feel awful about

themselves; they want to hide or disappear. Although

guilt is an uncomfortable emotion, shame can be

more debilitating. Phenomenological reports of

shame describe people feeling powerless and insignificant

(Wicker, Payne, & Morgan, 1983).

The

self-loathing that many traumatized women experience

may generalize into thinking of both the self and the

body as bad and ugly (Hyman, 1999; Miller, 1994). The

body, the site of the original abuse, was violated, and

this abuse included both emotional and physical

boundary violations. These boundary violations may

lead to self-harm, which temporarily helps define the

body boundaries.

Marital violence, for instance, is related to more depression and anxiety in school-age children (Hughes & Luke, 1998; McCloskey et al., 1995). Harsh punitive parenting and low parental warmth predict adolescent delinquency (Loeber & Dishion, 1983; McCord, 1997; Sampson & Laub, 1993; Simons, Wu, Johnson, & Conger, 1995), as does sexual abuse, especially among girls (Herrera

&McCloskey, 2003; Paolucci et al., 2001; Siegel &Williams, 2003).

Retrospective reports of parental emotional abusiveness, but not physical abusiveness, were related to shameproneness, whereas neither was related to guiltproneness (Hoglund & Nicholas, 1995).

In the past 20 years, the majority of research examining shame and guilt using the self- versus behavior distinction has focused on psychological adjustment. Researchers have found that after controlling for guilt, shame tends to be highly related to a variety of psychopathology, including PTSD, obsessive- compulsiveness, psychoticism, anxiety, and depression (Andrews et al., 2000; Ferguson, Stegge, Eyre, Vollmer, & Ashbaker, 2000; Harder, Cutler, & Rockart, 1992; Quiles & Bybee, 1997; Tangney et al., 1992).

Shame-proneness showed no association with criminal behavior but was linked to ensuing depression.

Guilt-proneness, on the other hand, showed little relation to depression but seemed to inhibit

engagement in criminal activities. Shame-proneness in early adolescence was associated

with symptoms of depression in late adolescence even when controlling for childhood symptoms of

depression. Although researchers have often found a concurrent relationship between shame-proneness and depression (Tangney et al., 1992), very few studies have looked at this prospectively.

The Relation of Child Maltreatment to Shame and Guilt Among Adolescents: Psychological Routes to Depression and Delinquency

Jeffrey Stuewig, George Mason University, Laura A. McCloskey, University of Pennsylvania

CHILDMALTREATMENT,Vol. 10, No. 4,November 2005 324-336

A common assumption is

that shame arises from thoughts that one has done something

bad or shameful, and the relationship between

abuse-specific internal attributions and shame is indeed

significant, both at the time of initial disclosure and 1 year

later (C. Feiring, personal communication, July 2001).

However, it is also possible for shame to arise from the

belief that one simply is bad or unworthy, independent of

any actions one has or has not taken. For example, abuserelated

shame could occur even in the absence of selfblame,

if children believe that the mere fact of having

been abused makes them bad, dirty, or shameful.

Addressing Attributions in Treating Abused Children - Judith A. Cohen, Anthony P. Mannarino - CHILD MALTREATMENT, Vol. 7, No. 1, February 2002 81-84

The articles in this special section on child maltreatment

and shame are efforts to elucidate the role of

shame as a contributor to child abuse consequences.

As many of the authors mention, most of the research

attention on emotions in child maltreatment—

especially with reference to posttraumatic stress—has

focused on fear as the negative emotion that produces

psychological distress in the aftermath of trauma.

More recently, researchers have begun to explore

other negative emotions that might arise during

or after abuse and that might factor into understanding

outcomes. Shame has emerged as a leading

candidate.

Although shame is the specific target of the investigations,

the primary contribution of these articles

may be in confirming that strong negative emotional

responses to abuse experiences beyond fear are

important in explaining outcomes. For example, why

does child sexual abuse produce such high rates of

post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) when most situations

do not involve fear-inducing events? While part

of the explanation may lie in the perception of life

threat as opposed to objective danger (a known predictor

for PTSD), perhaps there is something about

the nature of sexual trauma that is more likely to produce

other intense negative emotions. Because

shame and anger are associated with PTSD in adult

samples experiencing a variety of traumas (Andrews,

Brewin, Rose, & Kirk, 2000), more attention to these

emotions in child research is warranted. In addition,

of course, clinicians have long observed that strong

negative emotional responses that are not fear related

seem to be just as troubling for children who are traumatized.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIJE IN AFEKTI – REGULACIJA EMOCIJ IN AFEKTOV

All key participants talked about their use of alcohol and drugs to numb their emotional

pain from difficult and often painful life experiences and practices. All of the

key participants shared that they had discovered early on that alcohol and drugs

helped them to numb painful feelings from a past they had desperately tried to

escape.

The Mediating Effect of Emotion Regulation

Coinciding with the manifestation of other temperamental

characteristics, emotion regulation comprises a set of

competencies to modulate affective states (Shields & Cicchetti,

1998). Examples of emotion regulatory strategies

include self-soothing, reframing upsetting events and provocative

stimuli (Schwartz & Proctor, 2000), and inhibiting

or initiating emotionally driven behavior (Eisenberg et al.,

2001). These abilities are formed in the family context and

transferred to the peer realm (Fabes, Eisenberg, & Miller,

1990). Parents shape children’s acquisition of regulation

skills through parent– child interactions (Parke et al., 1992)

or by coaching and modeling (Carson & Parke, 1996; Davies

& Cummings, 1994). As noted by Eisenberg et al.

(1999), “parental coaching helps children to develop the

ability to inhibit negative affect, to self-sooth, and to focus

attention (including attention in social contexts)” (p. 514).

“Parents who exhibit hurtful and hostile negative emotions

frequently may model dysregulated behavior for children to

imitate” (Eisenberg et al., 2001, p. 488).

A number of empirical studies support the link between

the emotion regulatory abilities of parents and their children.

The available evidence suggests that there is a clear link

between parenting styles and children’s capacities for emotion

regulation.

Variously described as harsh, overreactive, emotionally

negative, coercive, and controlling and authoritarian (Arnold,

O’Leary, Wolff, & Acker, 1993; Deater-Deckard &

Dodge, 1997), the specific acts comprising a cluster of harsh

parenting behaviors include yelling, frequent negative commands,

name calling, overt expressions of anger, and physical

threats and aggression. These harsh parenting descriptions

can be summarized into categories of coercive acts and

negative emotion expressions. In other words, sometimes

parents hit their children when they are angry or emotionally

out of control (Patterson, 1982).

Studies

by emotion researchers also suggest the mediating effect of

children’s emotion regulation in channeling the effect of

emotion-related negative parenting practices on children’s

social adjustment.

A large number of

studies suggest that coercion and harshness from mothers’

parenting behaviors have a stronger effect on children than

do fathers’ behaviors (e.g., Denham et al., 2000).

The social learning

theory (e.g., Bandura & Walters, 1959) postulates that the

role modeling effect is facilitated by gender identification.

Thus, parenting behaviors should have stronger effects on

same-sex than opposite-sex children. A socialization theory

on gender role differentiations also predicts that parents in

general feel greater responsibility for the socialization of

same-sex children (Huston, 1983) and thus exert closer

control over them (Power & Shanks, 1989). However, because

most children spend more time with their mothers

than fathers (Russell & Russell, 1987) and because girls are

less rigid in gender stereotyping than boys (Ruble & Martin,

1998), the potential gender identification effect is expected

to be more evident with fathers and sons than with mothers

and daughters (Lytton & Romney, 1991). In relation to

harsh parenting and child aggression, a biological approach

would also predict different arousals and responses from

same-sex than opposite-sex parent–child pairings (Fabes,

1994).

Other mechanisms may also affect parent–child relations.

One that is also pertinent to the emotional channeling of

harsh parenting is that of attachment. Existing research

suggests that attachment security does not vary as a function

of a child’s gender (Ainsworth, 1973), especially in early

childhood involving child–mother attachment (Rosen &

Burke, 1999). Gender differences in attachment to fathers

and in related child–father relationships also do not seem to

appear until late childhood to adolescence (Lieberman,

Doyle, & Markiewicz, 1999). This attachment research is

also consistent with Davies and Cummings’s (1994) child

emotional security hypothesis. The emotional security theory

does not postulate gender differences in young children’s

emotional responses to inter-adult conflict (Davies &

Cummings, 1994). Cognitive differences in boys’ and girls’

coping with parental conflict have been observed only in

older children and adolescents (Davies & Cummings,

1994).

One of the most enduring problems associated with childhood abuse is difficulty in affect regulation. Under ideal circumstances, the emergence of emotion regulation skills is guided in development by caretakers through activities, such as labeling and interpreting emotional experiences, soothing activities, and role modeling of effective mood regulation (e.g., Malatesta & Haviland, 1982). Unfortunately, these important socializing experiences are disturbed in caretaking environments characterized by sustained physical and/or sexual abuse. Substantial research has shown that maltreated children, compared with nonmaltreated children, have more difficulty managing their emotions adaptively throughout childhood (e.g., Shields & Cicchetti, 1998; Shipman & Zeman, 2001). Similarly, as adults, victims of childhood abuse show difficulties in emotion regulation, especially in the context of interpersonal relationships. Several studies have demonstrated that, compared with women who have suffered first-time traumas as adults (e.g., rape, physical assault), childhood abuse victims have been found to have more difficulty managing anger, hostility, anxiety, and depression (Browne & Finkelhor, 1986) and report significantly more problems in interpersonal functioning in work, home, and social domains (Zlotnick, Zakriski, Shea, & Costello, 1996).

Survivors often experience uncontrolled

emotion as the most salient indicator that something is

deeply wrong.

Van der Kolk et al.

(1996) confirmed that affect dysregulation is a central

symptom for survivors. Feelings are enemies that arrive

unannounced, create havoc in the survivor’s internal

and external world, and then leave behind a dark hole

of nothingness that can feel even worse than the storm

that preceded it. The intense stress of the early trauma

can lead to overstimulation of the central nervous system.

In turn, this stimulation can cause permanent

neural changes that negatively affect learning and stimulus

discrimination (van der Kolk, 1994).

Alterations in biological functioning (easy to startle, chronic hyperarousal,

hypervigilance, and diffuse physical complaints)

are the hallmarks of many trauma survivors

(van der Kolk, 1994). Researchers have shown that individuals

with a history of trauma react to stimuli with conditioned

autonomic responses, such as increased heart rate and

increased blood pressure (Bremner, 1999). These responses

mimic the responses at the time of trauma.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – EMOCIJE IN AFEKTI – OBRAMBNI MEHANIZMI

The findings described thus far are consistent with

Bowlby’s (1980, 1987) notion of defensive exclusion.

Bowlby proposed that some individuals may selectively

and defensively regulate the processing of material that

could result in attachment-system activation. Such

defensive behavior may serve to prevent the negative

affect associated with reminders of attachment-related

loss. Avoidant individuals are thought to rely on these

kinds of defensive strategies to regulate attention to

attachment-related information: If potentially upsetting

information is not fully processed, the attachment system

is less likely to be activated, thus preventing further

rejection and distress and, of relevance to the present

study, impairing memory.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – VEDENJE (ACTING OUT), DELINKVENTNOST, NASILJE

Studies have found a possible link between an individual experiencing trauma and later developing delinquent behaviors. Burton, Foy, Bwanausi, Johnson, and Moore (1994) reported a significant level of PTSD in male juvenile delinquents adjudicated for felony crimes. Similarly, Steiner et al. (1997) reported a significantly high prevalence rate for PTSD within an incarcerated group of male juvenile delinquents when compared with males from a local high school who were not incarcerated. PTSD in male criminal youth was found to be associated with exposure to a malevolent environment such us domestic violence, living in unsafe neighborhoods, substance abuse, and criminal behavior among family members (Erwin et al., 2000).

There is strong evidence that child maltreatment (physical abuse, sexual abuse, and neglect) can increase risk for a range of negative adolescent behaviors, including delinquency, substance use, and violence (Fergusson, Horwood, & Lynskey, 1996; Fergusson & Lynskey, 1997; Hawkins et al., 1998; R. C. Herrenkohl, Egolf, & Herrenkohl, 1997; Smith & Thornberry, 1995;Widom, 1989a, 2000;Wolfe, 1999). Violence as an outcome of physical child abuse is, perhaps, most well studied, although results are inconsistent (Hawkins et al., 1998; Widom, 1989a, 1989b). In their comprehensive review of the research literature, Hawkins et al. (1998) found that effect sizes vary notably across studies linking maltreatment to official and self-reported violence in youths. Widom (1989a, 1989b) suggested that research design and measurement problems are common in studies on child maltreatment and that these problems explain, in part, why results differ.

In several longitudinal studies, the relation between physical child abuse and later violence does indeed appear to hold (Widom, 1989a). For example, in Widom’s (1989a, 1998a, 2000) longitudinal cohorts design study, results

show a strong link between physical child abuse and youth (and adult) violent crime arrests.

In that well-designed studies have shown a developmental link between physical child abuse and youth violence, a next step is to determine why. Several explanations for why abuse places children at risk for violence have been offered. One hypothesis is that youths simply reenact some version of the behavior to which they fell victim at the hands of an abusive parent, having learned the uses of violence (Widom, 2000). Dodge and colleagues (Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1990; Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1997; Dodge, Price, Bachorowski,&Newman, 1990) hypothesized instead that children who areabused suffer deficits in social information processing, which make them prone to violence. Their research suggests that abused children, through repeated exposure to severe punishment, develop a tendency to overattribute hostile intentions to others and to misread social cues in instances in which threats are unclear or even unintended. In this process, a youth assumes the role of an aggressor against a perceived hostile target.

Results of the prospective abuse model showed that the parental attachment and school commitment variables played lesser a role in the mediation of abuse on later violence than did violent attitudes and peer involvement. In this model, abuse predicted violent attitudes, which, in turn, predicted involvement with antisocial peers, which, in turn, predicted violent behavior in youths. The link between earlier abuse and violent attitudes in youths is indeed suggestive of a cognitive/social learning perspective on the transmission of violence, in which youths incorporate into their own cognitive and social interactional styles ways of relating that resemble those used by their parents toward them.

POSLEDICE TRAVME – NEVROPSIHOLOŠKE POSLEDICE

Early experiences with a traumatizing caregiver are well known to impact negatively the child’s attachment security, stress coping strategies, and sense of self (Crittenden and Ainsworth, 1989; Erickson, Egeland, and Pianta, 1989). (Advances in Neuropsychoanalysis, Attachment Theory, and Trauma Research: Implications for Self Psychology, ALLAN N. SCHORE, PH.D.)

Current studies in developmental traumatology conclude that “the overwhelming stress of maltreatment in childhood is associated with adverse influences on brain development” (de Bellis et al., 1999, p. 1281). And so it is now thought that specifically a dysfunctional and traumatized early relationship is the stressor that leads to PTSD, that severe trauma of interpersonal origin may override any genetic, constitutional, social, or psychological resilience factor, and that the ensuing adverse effects on brain development and alterations of the biological stress systems may be regarded as “an environmentally induced complex developmental disorder” (de Bellis, 2001). (Advances in Neuropsychoanalysis, Attachment Theory, and Trauma Research: Implications for Self Psychology, ALLAN N. SCHORE, PH.D.)

During the first two years of life, chronic and cumulative states of overwhelming, hyperaroused affective states, as well as hypoaroused dissociation have devastating effects on the growth of psychic structure. The survival mode of conservation-withdrawal induces an extreme alteration of the bioenergetics of the developing brain. In critical periods of regional synaptogenesis this would have growth-inhibiting effects, especially in the right brain, which specializes in withdrawal. This is because the biosynthetic processes that mediate the proliferation of synaptic connections in the postnatally developing brain demand, in addition to sufficient quantities of essental nutrients, massive amounts of energy. An infant brain that is chronically shifting into hypometabolic survival modes has little energy available for growth (see Schore, 1994,

1997a, 2001b).

Recent neurobiological studies in developmental traumatology indicate that the infant’s psychobiological response to trauma is comprised of two separate response patterns, hyperarousal and dissociation (Perry et al., 1995; Schore, 1998d, 1999b, c, 2001b, e, f, 2002e). In the initial stage of threat, an alarm reaction is initiated, in which the sympathetic

component of the ANS is suddenly and significantly activated, resulting in increased heart rate, blood pressure, and respiration. Distress is expressed in crying and then screaming. This state of fear-terror is mediated by sympathetic hyperarousal, and it reflects increased levels of the major stress hormone corticotropin releasing factor, which in turn regulates noradrenaline and adrenaline activity (see Schore, 1997a, 2001b, 2002e). But a second later-forming, longer-lasting traumatic reaction is seen in dissociation, in which the child disengages from stimuli in the external world and attends to an “internal” world. The child’s dissociation in the midst of terror involves numbing, avoidance, compliance,

and restricted affect. Traumatized infants are observed to be “staring off into space with a glazed look.” This parasympathetic dominant state of conservation-withdrawal occurs in helpless and hopeless stressful situations in which the individual becomes inhibited and strives to avoid attention in order to become “unseen” (Kaufman and Rosenblum, 1967;

Schore, 1994). This primary regulatory process for maintaining organismic homeostasis

(Engel and Schmale, 1972) is characterized by a metabolic shutdown (Schore, 2001b, in press a) and low levels of activity (McCabe and Schneiderman, 1985). (Advances in Neuropsychoanalysis, Attachment Theory, and Trauma Research: Implications for Self Psychology, ALLAN N. SCHORE, PH.D.)

Very recent basic research indicates that maternal deprivation increases cell death in the infant brain (Zhang et al., 2002). Is this the death instinct? Recall, the state of conservation-withdrawal, a primary regulatory process of decreased metabolic energy is accessed when active coping (flight or fight) is not possible, occurs in hopeless and helpless contexts, and is behaviorally manifest as feigning death (Engel and Schmale, 1972; Powles, 1992).

(Attachment Theory, and Trauma Research: Implications for Self Psychology, ALLAN N. SCHORE, PH.D.)

Functions of the mind that spoil what we call ‘intimacy’ will deepen and darken the

‘black hole of trauma’ (van der Kolk & McFarlane, 1996). It is also known that

secondary stressors play an increasingly in? uential role in the development of a

trauma reaction progressively over time and damaged capacity for intimacy

represents an important potential secondary stressor.

C

Posttraumatic stress effects on child witnesses have also been studied but, again, not as frequently as one might expect. Witnessing domestic violence between parents has been said to qualify as a traumatic stressor insofar as it entails the deliberate harm or threat of serious harm to a loved one and a profound sense of helplessness for child witnesses.

CHILDREN EXPOSED TO INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE

Research Findings and Implications for Intervention

BONNIE E. CARLSON

State University of New York at Albany

TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 1, No. 4, October 2000 321-342

Research findings do not support hippocampal shrinkage in children, although there is some

evidence of generalized lower brain volumes. DeBellis and colleagues (DeBellis, Baum, et al., 1999; DeBellis,Keshavan, et al., 2002) failed to find hippocampal volume loss in maltreated children and adolescents compared with controls but did find other brain differences such as lower overall cerebral

volume, lower corpus callosum volume, and greater ventricular and cerebral fluid volumes in the maltreated group. Similar results were found by Carrion, Weems, Eliez, et al. (2001) who observed significantly smaller overall brain and cerebral volumes in children with a history of repeated exposure to trauma compared with controls. Trauma-exposed children had smaller intracranial, cerebral, and prefrontal cortex, and prefrontal cortical white matter, as well as greater frontal lobe fluid volume than controls.

Advances and Future Directions in the Study of Children’s Neurobiological Responses to Trauma and Violence Exposure

KATHERINE BEVANS, Tulane University, ARLEEN B. CERBONE, Louisiana State University, STACY OVERSTREET, Tulane University

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 4, April 2005 418-425

Stress response is largely mediated by two interrelated systems, the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine (LC/NE) system and the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis (Lupien & McEwen, 1997). However, most neurobiological research on child exposure to violence and trauma concerns HPA-axis

stress response (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2001; Hart, Gunnar, & Cicchetti, 1996). The HPA axis is a complex system of interrelated, multiple structural regions and neuromodulators in the brain designed to mediate the fight-flight response. Basal activity of the HPA axis, which is essential for normal brain growth and metabolic activity, follows a circadian rhythm with high earlymorning cortisol levels declining to low levels around the onset of sleep (McEwen, 1998).

Neuroimaging is another promising area of research into the effects of violence and trauma on the developing brain. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) can be used to identify gross pathological abnormalities as well as to quantify sizes of various brain regions. MRI is superior to computed tomographic (CT) scanning because of the relatively inferior soft tissue resolution seen in CT.

Advances and Future Directions in the Study of Children’s Neurobiological Responses to Trauma and Violence Exposure

KATHERINE BEVANS, Tulane University, ARLEEN B. CERBONE, Louisiana State University, STACY OVERSTREET, Tulane University

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 4, April 2005 418-425

One of the most exciting developments to emerge from the field in the past 20 years is the increasing attention to neurobiological responses to violence and trauma exposure. Although researchers have yet to identify a consensual pattern of neurobiological response to violence and trauma exposure, it does appear that some type of alteration in the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis is likely. This article briefly reviews the multiple moderating factors that help account for the divergent patterns in HPA function as well as methodological advances that will continue to improve the assessment of HPA function in youth exposed to violence and trauma.

Advances and Future Directions in the Study of Children’s Neurobiological Responses to Trauma and Violence Exposure

KATHERINE BEVANS, Tulane University, ARLEEN B. CERBONE, Louisiana State University, STACY OVERSTREET, Tulane University

JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE, Vol. 20 No. 4, April 2005 418-425

A number of researchers maintain that these two criteria preclude PTSD and

amnesia from coexisting, and some empirical studies support this position [7± 10].

Sbordone [7] noted that the typical PTSD patient cannot erase the traumatic event

from his/her mind and suffers recurrent intrusions and re-experiencing of the event,

symptoms that are incompatible with amnesia. He further argued that amnesia,

commonly resulting from cerebral contusion, is an impairment of the ability to

process and record ongoing events. Therefore, in the aftermath of the trauma,

there are no memories of the event for the amnesic patient to re-experience.

Despite the theoretical arguments against co-occurring PTSD and amnesia, a

number of case reports and empirical studies have indicated that amnesic victims can

develop PTSD [1, 11± 13]. The present study prospectively examined the incidence

of PTSD in amnesic and non-amnesic MVA victims.

BIOLOŠKE ZNAČILNOSTI

Recent studies have demonstrated that PTSD patients differ in basal neuroendocrine

levels from trauma victims without PTSD. For example, PTSD patients

have exhibited greater levels of 24-hour urinary catecholamine excretion than

controls [14± 16], and a majority of studies (for exceptions see [17, 18]) have

found lower 24-hour urinary cortisol excretion in patients with PTSD compared

to trauma victims without PTSD and controls [14, 19± 23]. Heightened basal levels

of catecholamines suggest a general hyperarousal of sympathetic nervous system

(SNS) activity, while lower cortisol levels indicate an accompanying down-regulation

of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. In addition, combining SNS and

HPA hormone levels into a NE/cortisol ratio has demonstrated greater diagnostic

specificity for PTSD than either hormone alone [24]. These findings have led

researchers to suggest that greatly increased catecholamine levels without the

compensatory effect of accompanying cortisol increases at the time of a traumatic event could lead to the formation of `superconditioned’ memories that may become

the basis of intrusive thoughts and the re-experiencing symptoms of PTSD [25].

The majority of these studies have examined neuroendocrine abnormalities in

chronic PTSD patients who have often been symptomatic for over 20 years and

suffered comorbid drug or alcohol abuse. More recent research has reported that

MVA victims who met PTSD criteria 1 month following the accident excreted

significantly lower levels of cortisol in 15-hour urine samples collected upon admission

to the trauma unit than non-PTSD victims [2]. This suggests that initial

physiological responses to trauma may contribute, in part, to the development of

PTSD following a traumatic event. If amnesic patients experience the traumatic

event differently from non-amnesics, this may provide a mechanism through which

amnesia could buffer post-traumatic stress. The present study examined initial neuroendocrine

response and the development of PTSD symptomatology in amnesic and

non-amnesic MVA victims. It was hypothesized that amnesic patients would be less

likely to meet PTSD diagnostic criteria 1 month after their accident and would

display lower catecholamine levels and higher basal cortisol than non-amnesic

victims. Further, it was hypothesized that initial hormone levels would mediate

the relationship between amnesia and PTSD.

REZULTATI

Of the 53 participants who were assessed at the 1-month follow-up, nine

individuals (17%) met full PTSD diagnostic criteria. Chi-square analyses revealed

that non-amnesics were more likely to meet diagnostic criteria than amnesics

(À2…1; n ˆ 53† ˆ 4:85; p < 0:05). None of the amnesic subjects met criteria for

PTSD.

The present study provides partial support for the hypothesis that amnesia for a

traumatic event can serve as a buffering function in the development of subsequent PTSD among MVA victims. None of the amnesic patients in the sample met PTSD

criteria due to their accident, and amnesic patients reported fewer symptoms of

PTSD on the IES and on the SCID. Additionally, amnesics and non-amnesics

differed in their initial physiological responses to the MVA, with amnesics having

lower NE/cortisol ratios than non-amnesics. These results suggest that amnesics

may physiologically experience the accident differently from non-amnesics, and

that amnesics have lower subsequent PTSD incidence. However, urinary hormones

did not mediate the effects of amnesia on PTSD, although this may have been due

to the lack of power afforded by the small sample size.

The findings of lower NE/cortisol ratio in amnesics compared to non-amnesics

may provide some insight into the protectiveness of amnesia in subsequent PTSD

development. Prior research has found that heightened catecholamine levels are

associated with greater memory retention of stressful stimuli in animal and

human studies [38, 39]. In addition, other researchers have hypothesized that

high levels of catecholamines without the compensatory effects of cortisol could

lead to aberrant memory formation or `supermemories’ [25]. These results support

this hypothesis, as it was found that amnesics who had a combination of low NE

and high cortisol (without differences in either alone) also had a lower incidence of

PTSD, and fewer PTSD symptoms, than non-amnesics.

Traumatic experiences cause traumatic stress, which disrupts homeostasis.

During the past few years, we have dramatically increased our understanding

of the effects of traumatic stress on the brain, sympathetic nervous

system, and endocrine system. Through a physiological domino effect, these

changes affect many other body systems, including the cardiovascular system,

respiratory system, and muscular system.

The endocrine system works closely with the nervous system to regulate

the body’s physiology. Traumatic experience causes both immediate and

long-term endocrine changes that affect metabolism and neurophysiology.

The sympathetic nervous system is immediately affected by any perception

of danger and signals the adrenal medulla to greatly increase its output of epi-

nephrine and norepinephrine. These hormones rapidly affect many body systems

leading to a fight-or-flight response. If neither response is possible, the

person freezes.

During stress, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal system is also activated,

leading to increased levels of cortisol, a glucocorticoid released by the

adrenal cortex that modulates the physiologic response to stress and helps

activate effective coping strategies. Cortisol concentration has been the focus

of several recent studies because abnormally high levels of cortisol, associated

with stress, can damage neurons in the hippocampus.

Abnormal concentrations of adrenal hormones depress the immune system

and contribute to the physiological hyperarousal (e.g., exaggerated startle

response, hypervigilance) characteristic of PTSD. Chronic physiological

hyperarousal makes it very difficult to regulate autonomic responses to internal

or external signals and decreases the ability to respond appropriately to

emotional signals (van der Kolk, 1996). In chronic stress and in Post-Traumatic

Stress Disorder (PTSD), cortisol concentrations are lower than would

be expected, and exposure to new stressors elicits lower levels of cortisol

secretion.

To understand the problems presented by traumatic memories, we will

first review how nontraumatic memories are processed. Memories of ordinary

experiences are temporarily stored in the limbic system as episodic

memories, memories of personal experience and events. Episodic memories

are autobiographical; they include a sense of time and self. Cognitive aspects

are stored in the hippocampus and the associated emotion is stored in the

amygdala. As the brain processes these memories over time, aspects of them

are abstracted and transferred to the neocortex, particularly the association

areas of the frontal lobes, for long-term storage. These memories are seman-

tic, or factual, memories. (Episodic and semantic memories are two types of

explicit memory.)

The memories of moderately disturbing experiences apparently remain in

the right limbic system for a longer period of time than the memories of neutral

events.We process disturbing memories by thinking, talking, and sometimes

dreaming about the experience. As the brain slowly processes the

memory, it is abstracted and transferred into the left neocortex where it is

filed away along with other memories and becomes part of the narrative of

one’s life. The stored information can be retrieved when needed to understand

future events.

Traumatic events overwhelm the brain’s capacity to process information.

The episodic memory of the experience may be dysfunctionally stored in the

right limbic system indefinitely and may generate vivid images of the traumatic

experience, terrifying thoughts, feelings, body sensations, sounds, and

smells. Such unprocessed traumatic memories can cause cognitive and emotional

looping, anxiety, PTSD, maladaptive coping strategies, depression,

and many other psychological symptoms of distress. Because the episodic

memory is not processed, a relevant semantic memory is not stored and

the individual has difficulty using knowledge from the experience to guide

future action.

Because traumatic experiences are terrifying, the survivor avoids thinking

and talking about what happened. This avoidance prevents processing.

Trauma alters physiology and gives rise to images, feelings, sensations, and

beliefs that may persist throughout life. Only after the traumatic memory is

fully processed and integrated can homeostasis be restored.

Traumatic memories can be triggered by stimuli that are in some way

associated with the traumatic event. Terrifying memories, including the

affect associated with them, may be reexperienced with their original intensity.

Survivors feel the terror and may lose their sense of time and place. One

client stated, “Part of me knows it’s not really happening now, but it feels so

real that I get mixed up.”

Brain scan technology enables us to study the brain in action. Using PET

scans, researchers have demonstrated some of the neurophysiological effects

that take place when traumatic memory is triggered. In one study, participants

were asked to write detailed narratives of their traumatic experience

(Rauch et al., 1996). Then, each participant was asked to read the narrative

during brain scanning. The results were dramatic. Activity increased in the

right brain, primarily in the limbic system and in the visual cortex (the site

of vivid images of the event). Activity decreased in the anterior cingulate

cortex (ACC), which normally modulates the limbic system. Activity also

decreased in Broca’s area, an area of the brain important in semantic processing

and articulation of language. This decrease in activity may be the

neurophysiological basis for the “speechless terror” that many individuals

experience both during a traumatic event and when processing trauma in

therapy.

In the United States, child abuse and neglect are the most common causes

of Type III trauma, extreme trauma characterized by multiple traumatic experiences

that typically begin at an early age (Solomon & Heide, 1999). An

infant’s relationship with its primary caregiver has a direct effect on the hard

wiring of neural circuits in the developing brain. Many of the neural circuits

affected by early experience connect areas of the brain critical for emotional,

physiological, psychological, and social development. Some of these circuits

are necessary for adaptive coping in emotional and stressful situations

(Schore, 2003).

The orbitofrontal cortex helps regulate emotional states and responses. By

way of its connections with the hypothalamus and limbic system, it regulates

autonomic responses to social stimuli and mediates emotionally “attuned

communication.” This part of the cortex helps us understand other people’s

emotional experience, enabling us to respond empathically, a capacity necessary

for moral judgment. Normal development of the right brain and later

emotional and social development depend on healthy attachment between

infant and caregiver (Schore, 1994, 1996, 2003).

Children who are severely neglected experience chronic traumatic stress

that compromises right brain development, resulting in neuron damage and

atrophy. Impairment of the orbitofrontal cortex and the circuits connecting it

with subcortical areas can diminish the child’s sense of self, leading to disconnection

from other people. Severely neglected children do not deal well

with stress and do not develop the ability to regulate the intensity and duration

of their affect (Schore, 2002; van der Kolk & Fisler, 1994). Because

these children have difficulty understanding emotion expressed by other people,

they may not develop empathy.

Many studies conclude that impaired development of the orbitofrontal

cortex and its neural connections with the limbic system decreases capacity

to regulate affect. For example, the orbitofrontal cortex normally inhibits

areas in the hypothalamus that are associated with aggression and thus is central

in the regulation of aggressive impulses. Abnormal development of the

neural circuits linking the orbitofrontal cortex and ACC with the amygdala

interferes with normal inhibition of rage responses.Without the normal cor-

Solomon, Heide / THE BIOLOGY OF TRAUMA 55

tical modulating effect, the amygdala’s responses are exaggerated. When

aggressive impulses are not inhibited, an individual may act out violently.

This lack of inhibition is part of the pattern of sociopathy (Best,Williams,&

Coccaro, 2002; Schore, 2003). Studies suggest that trauma caused by neglect

and abuse can lead to antisocial behavior (Heide, 1992, 1999).

Many long-term changes in the brain have been associated with Type III

trauma, including abnormal concentrations of certain neurotransmitters,

changes in EEG patterns, and a decrease in integration between right and left

hemispheres. Measurable size decreases have been found in the cerebral volume,

the corpus callosum, amygdala, and hippocampus. Whether or not

these changes are reversible with treatment is an important question for

future study.

NEVROPSIHOLOŠKE RAZLIKE MED SPOLOMA

[pic]

Fig. 1. The amygdala (red region), a small almond-shaped

structure located deep in the anterior temporal lobe, plays a

critical role in a variety of emotional processes including emotional

memory and adaptive responses to emotional stimuli.

Recent work suggests that several differences between men

and women in emotional responses arise in part from sex differences

in amygdala responses. Reprinted with permission by

Digitial Anatomist Project, Department of Biological Structure,

University of Washington.

In addition to functional differences in amygdala

response, such as in emotional memory and in

responses to sexually arousing stimuli, the amygdala

in men and women differs in terms of structure and in

aspects of brain development. These structural and

developmental differences likely contribute to the

functional differences observed in neuroimaging

studies.

One major difference between the sexes is the size

of the amygdala. In the adult human brain, the male

amygdala is significantly larger than the female

amygdala, even when total brain size is taken into

account (Goldstein and others 2001). Although the

specific consequences of this sex difference in amygdala

size are not known, structural differences in brain

anatomy often are associated with differences in brain

function and response. For example, one recent study

found a relation between the size of the amygdala in

patients with epilepsy and sexual drive; patients with

greater residual amygdala size after undergoing neurosurgery

reported greater sexual drive and motivation

(Baird and others 2004). Interestingly, the brain

regions that differ in size between men and women

tend also to be the same regions that contain high

concentrations of sex hormone receptors, suggesting

that male and female hormones play a role in determining

the size of specific brain regions such as the

amygdala during brain development (Goldstein and

others 2001). Consistent with this idea, neuroimaging

studies have found that amygdala, which contains relatively

high concentrations of sex hormone receptors,

develops structurally at different rates in human males

and females. Other structural differences in areas that

receive strong neuronal connections from the amygdala,

such as the hypothalamus, which is larger in men

than women, may also contribute to sex differences in

brain response that involve the amygdala. Circulating

levels of sex hormones in the bloodstream constitute

an additional influence on amygdala response through

their action on receptor sites. Future work will be necessary

to elucidate the complex relationship between

structural, developmental, and functional aspects of

amygdala sex differences.

Memory for emotional events is generally better than

memory for emotionally neutral events (Hamann 2001).

Several psychological studies have reported that men

and women differ substantially with respect to emotional

memory (Hamann and Canli 2004). For example,

women can recall emotional memories more quickly, can

recall more emotional memories in a given period of

time, and report that the emotional memories they recall

are richer, more vivid, and more intense. In general,

then, women tend to experience greater enhancement of

their memory by emotion (Seidlitz and Diener 1998).

The stronger effect of emotion on women’s memories is

not entirely beneficial, however. As described below,

emotion can also impair memory in some situations, and

this impairment is accentuated in women. In addition,

the fact that emotional memories tend to be stronger for

women may be linked to the greater prevalence of

depression and some types of anxiety disorders in

women (Davidson and others 2002).

The three neuroimaging studies that have examined

the brain correlates of these differences in emotional

memories have found a remarkably consistent pattern of

sex differences in the role of the left and right amygdala

in emotional memory. These studies have focused on the

effects of emotional arousal on declarative memory,

memory for facts or events that can be brought to mind

through a conscious, voluntary effort to retrieve the

memory (Squire and Zola 1996). Each of these studies

examined differences in brain activity occurring during

memory encoding (i.e., memory formation) that were

predictive of subsequent successful emotional memory

retrieval. That is, one can examine which items are successfully

retrieved on a later test and then go back to

determine which brain areas were more active when

those items were originally encoded in the brain scanner.

Cahill and others (2001) used PET to image brain

activity while men and women watched either highly

aversive films or neutral films. The level of amygdala

activity at encoding predicted later emotional memory

performance for both males and females. However, for

females, this relation was found in the left amygdala

whereas for males it was in the right amygdala. A later

study by Canli and others (2002) examined brain activity

in men and women during the encoding of emotional

and neutral scenes in photographs, using fMRI.

Consistent with the prior PET study, amygdala activity

during the encoding of the most emotionally arousing

photographs was strongly related to later recognition

memory for the emotional pictures, but again this relationship

was seen in the left amygdala for women and

the right amygdala for men. The strength of an emotional

experience, referred to as emotional arousal, is currently

thought to be the most important factor that determines

the degree of memory enhancement associated

with an emotional event (Hamann and others 1999;

Canli and others 2000). In this study, participants’ rat-

ings of emotional arousal correlated with left amygdala

activity in both men and women. Thus, in females the

brain regions involved in emotional reactions coincide

with those involved in encoding memory for the experience,

whereas in males these processes occur in different

hemispheres. The authors suggested the greater overlap

between the neural correlates of emotional experience

and emotional memory in women as a possible explanation

for the greater vividness and accuracy of their emotional

memories.

In the third study, Cahill and others (2004) examined

memory for emotionally arousing photographs in men

and women using a similar fMRI task as was used by

Canli and others (2002) and again found that later levels

of emotional memory were strongly correlated with left

amygdala activity in women but right amygdala activity

in men. In summary, these studies have found a consistent

sex difference between the role of the left and right

amygdalas (Fig. 2). Speculations regarding the origin of

these sex differences have included sexually dimorphic

brain development (Goldstein and others 2001), the

influence of sex hormones both during development and

during adulthood, and possible differences in the cognitive

style used by men and women in encoding emotional

experiences.

Strange and others (2003) examined the effect of

inserting an emotional event (an emotionally arousing

word) into a sequence of neutral events (a list of neutral

words). As expected, memory for the arousing word was

better than memory for the neutral words. Interestingly,

however, memory for words presented just before the

emotional word was also affected, but instead of

enhancement, memory was impaired. This impairment

was not found in a patient with bilateral amygdala

lesions, strongly suggesting that the effect is amygdaladependent.

In addition, the size of the emotion-induced

memory impairment was found to be twice as large for

women than for men. The specific mechanisms responsible

for this memory impairment are yet unknown, but

these findings are noteworthy in that they suggest that

both the enhancing and the impairing effects of emotion

are magnified in women.

MERJENJE

Life Experiences Questionnaire (LEQ)

The LEQ is a self-report instrument developed by the second author

(Long, 1999), which includes questions regarding demographics and childhood

sexual experiences. Participants were instructed to report all sexual

experiences occurring before the age of 17.

Modified Sexual Experiences Survey (MSES)

The MSES is a modified version of the 10-item Sexual Experiences Survey

(SES) (Koss&Gidycz, 1985) andwas used to assess adult unwanted sexual

contact. The MSES asks a series of yes/no questions assessing whether

specific types of sexual activities had been attempted or completed with the

participant since the age of 17.

Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS)

The CTS Form N developed by Straus (1979) was used to assess the presence

and extent of intimate violence among dating partners and spouses.

Psychological Maltreatment of Women Inventory (PMWI)

Tolman’s (1989) PMWI was used to assess psychological abuse and contains

two subscales.

Abuse questionnaire

The next questions are about forms of childhood abuse to

which you may have been exposed to before 16 years of age.

1. Do you think that there was any kind of emotional neglect?

(This means for example that people at home didn’t listen to

you, that your problems were ignored, that you had the

feeling of not being able to find any attention or support

from the people in your house).

2. Do you think there was any kind of psychological abuse?

(This means for example; being sworn at, lesser treatment

compared to brothers or sisters, unjustified punishment,

blackmail).

3. Do you think there was any kind of physical abuse?

(That is, were you ever beaten, kicked, punched or did you

experience any other kind of physical abuse?

4. Were you ever approached sexually against your will?

(This means: were you ever been touched sexually by

anyone against your will or forced to touch anybody,

were you ever pressured into sexual contact against your

will?)

INSTRUMENTI ZA MERJENJE SIMPTOMOV KOMPLEKSNE TRAVME

Posttraumatic symptoms, PTSD, and CPTSD.[pic]

The following instruments are recommended at this time: Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS; Blake et al., 1996), Impact of Event Scale—Revised (IES–R; Weiss & Marmar, 1997), Detailed Assessment of Posttraumatic States (DAPS; Briere, 2001), and Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS; Foa, 1995). Perhaps the two most useful in the identification of CPTSD are the Trauma Symptom Inventory (TSI), an instrument developed to assess trauma symptoms proper but that assesses domains of the self and relations with others (Briere, 1995; Briere, Elliot, Harris, & Cotman, 1995), and the Structured Interview for Disorders of Extreme Stress (SIDES), developed for the DSM–IV field trial (Pelcovitz et al., 1997; van der Kolk, 1999; Zlotnick & Pearlstein, 1997). Additionally, the Inventory of Altered Self Capacities (IASC; Briere, 2000b) assesses difficulties in relatedness, identity, and affect regulation and is therefore very pertinent to this population, as do the Cognitive Distortion Scales (CDS; Briere, 2000a) and the Trauma and Attachment Belief Scale (Pearlman, 2003), measures of trauma-related beliefs and cognitive distortions.

NEOPREDELJENO

................
................

In order to avoid copyright disputes, this page is only a partial summary.

Google Online Preview   Download