Behavior Problems in Children and Adolescents with Visual ...



Behaviour Problems in Children and Adolescents

with Visual Impairment[1]

Michael Brambring

University of Bielefeld (Germany)

Behaviour problems: Definitions and approaches

Usually, behaviour problems are assessed with two different approaches. The first is statistical judgment; that is, a behaviour disorder is diagnosed if the child or adolescent exhibits extremely frequent or extremely severe maladjusted behaviour in comparison with other children or adolescents of the same age, gender, or sociocultural group. This comparison means, for example, that separation anxiety from the mother will usually be judged as normal in 3-year-olds, because most children of this age exhibit such behaviour, but the same behaviour will usually be judged as maladjusted in 10-year-olds. Whereas, stereotypies may be judged as a disturbed behaviour in the sighted, this does not necessarily hold for the blind population, because most blind children exhibit them.

The second kind of assessment is called clinical judgment, that is, behaviour problems are problems that affect physical, mental, and psychological well-being to a clinically significant degree. This assessment of the impact on the child or adolescent's current or prospective developmental potentials is independent of how many children or adolescents exhibit comparable difficulties.

Assessment methods

In the statistical approach, assessments are carried out with questionnaires such as the well-known behaviour checklists published by Achenbach and colleagues (Achenbach, 1985).

(Child Behaviour Checklist for Ages 2 - 3 (CBCL/2-3)

(Child Behaviour Checklist for Ages 4 - 18 (CBCL/4-18)

(Teachers' Report Form for Ages 5 - 18 (TRF/5-18)

(Youth Self Report for Ages 11 - 18 (YSR/11-18)

The statistical judgment is based on critical cut-off points, that is, when the number of behaviour problems exceeds statistical norms. For example, a behaviour problem is given only if less than 5 % of the comparison group exhibit this type of maladjusted behaviour. This approach raises the methodological problem that almost all questionnaires are standardized for the sighted population, meaning a risk of invalid items for children with visual impairment. An example is "avoids eye contact" in CBCL/2-3. There is only one standardized questionnaire for adolescents with visual impairment, the so-called "Adolescent Emotional Factors Inventory" (Bauman, 1964).

Clinical judgments, in contrast, are based on two internationally widespread classification systems:

(The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)

(American Psychiatric Association, 1994)

(The International Classification of Diseases (ICD 10). Classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines

(World Health Organization, 1992)

Criteria for clinically relevant disorders are based on the frequency, severity, duration, and generalizability of the behaviour problems across situations. When applying these classification systems to the assessment of behaviour problems in children or adolescents with visual impairment, one has to be aware that such criteria for judging clinically relevant disorders are almost always based on experiences with sighted children.

Theories of behaviour problems

Many theories offer explanations for the emergence of behaviour disorders from a biological, psychodynamic, or learning theory perspective (Gelfand, Jenson, & Drew, 1997). One well-known model for the explanation of behaviour problems is the so-called diathesis-stress or vulnerability-stress hypothesis. This assumes that behaviour problems emerge if there is an imbalance between the biological or acquired competencies of the child and the demands of the physical and social environment. It predicts that behaviour problems emerge in "normal" children when physical and social demands exceed their competencies to a large extent. In contrast, one may expect even regular demands to exceed the competencies of children with impairments. Thus, the model would suggest a higher risk of behaviour problems in this group.

Prevalence of behaviour problems

(1) Normal population. The prevalence of behaviour problems in the sighted population may vary for many reasons such as the type of assessment method or the assessor. The famous Isle-of-Wight Study (Rutter, Graham, & Yule, 1970), assessing almost 11,000 children and adolescents, yielded an overall prevalence of behaviour problems in the normal population of 10-15% mild or middle-graded behaviour problems and 7% middle-graded to severe problems (i. e., psychiatric disturbance). Other empirical studies converge to produce major conclusions on age and gender differences as well as differences in the kind of behaviour problems in both sexes:

(Age differences:

Findings yield a slight increase in behaviour problems from preschool age to adolescence.

(Gender differences:

(1) No significant gender differences are found at preschool age.

(2) Behaviour problems are more frequent in boys than in girls at school age.

(3) In contrast, there are more frequent behaviour problems in girls than in boys during adolescence.

(Kind of behaviour problems:

(1) Boys exhibit a predominance of so-called externalized behaviour problems such as attention and hyperactivity deficiency, aggression, or oppositional behaviour.

(2) Girls exhibit a predominance of so-called internalized behaviour problems such as anxiety, depression, social withdrawal, or psychosomatic complaints.

(2) Prevalence in children or adolescents with visual impairment. The prevalence of behaviour problems in children or adolescents with visual impairment can be ascertained from two studies with almost representative samples. First, the Vancouver study (Jan, Freeman, & Scott, 1977), and second, the Israel study (Tirosh, Shnitzer, Davidovitch, & Cohen, 1998). Both studies were carried out at regional children's hospitals responsible for the treatment of children with visual impairments. Assessments performed by experienced pediatricians, child psychiatrists, or psychologists referred to the judgment of psychiatrically disturbed behaviour problems.

Table 1 reports the sample characteristics of both studies.

Table 1: Sample Characteristics

|Characteristic |Vancouver |Israel |

|Number of subjects |92 |193 |

| | | |

|Age |1-19 years |13 mo-8 years |

| | | |

|Visual impairment | | |

|Blind |27% |32.5% |

|Partially sighted |71% |67.5% |

| | | |

|Additional impairments | | |

|Mental retardation |20% |18% |

|Cerebral palsy |19% |12.5% |

|Hearing impairment |15% |12.4% |

|Epilepsy |15% |10.8% |

The percentages in Table 1 indicate comparable characteristics except for the participants' age distribution. Participants in the Vancouver study were older than those in the Israel study.

Table 2 reports the prevalence of behaviour problems in children and adolescents with visual impairment.

Table 2: Prevalence of Behaviour Problems

|Assessor |Vancouver (1977) |Israel (1998) |

|Parents/Teachers |61% |XXX |

|Psychiatrist/Psychologist |57% |49% |

There is a high level of agreement between the different assessors in the Vancouver study (parents/teachers vs. psychiatrist/psychologist) as well as a high level of agreement between both studies despite a 20-year interval, different age ranges, and different cultural settings.

Table 3 reports the findings of a follow-up study carried out by Freeman, Goetz, Richard, and Groenveld (1991).

Table 3: Follow-up Study at Vancouver 1991 (Freeman et al., 1991)

|Prevalence |Main study |Follow-up study |

| |(1977) |(1991) |

|Overall |57% |49% |

|Blind |68% |52% |

|Partially sighted |51% |48% |

|Visually impaired | | |

|without mental retardation |47% |32% |

Results indicate a decrease in the prevalence of behaviour problems from childhood /adolescence to adolescence/adulthood, particularly in the group of those who are blind or visually impaired without mental retardation. Nonetheless, the prevalence still remains much higher than that in the normal population.

Autism in children with congenital blindness

A new and controversial debate has arisen over autism in children who are congenitally blind. Autism is a pervasive disorder that includes disturbance in social interaction and communicative competencies as well as severe restrictions in activities, interests, and fantasies. The prevalence in the sighted population is approximately 4-5 per 10,000 (0.004-0.005%).

Findings on the prevalence of autism in the blind population vary greatly as a function of (a) the experiences of the assessor with blind-specific idiosyncrasies, (b) the explanation of these blind-specific idiosyncrasies, and (c) the chosen method, that is, clinical or statistical judgment.

The above-mentioned studies by Jan, Freeman, and Scott (1977), and Tirosh, Shnitzer, Davidovitch, and Cohen (1998)—based on clinical judgment—yielded prevalence rates of 3% (Jan et al., 1977) and 1% (Tirosh et al., 1998). Although these figures are still higher than those in the sighted population, they are much lower than the prevalence in newly published studies based on the statistical approach. Goodman and Minne (1995) assessed 17 congenitally blind 4- to 11-year-olds without any additional impairment with the Autism Behaviour Checklist (Krug, Arick, & Almond, 1978). Autism was diagnosed when the number of symptoms exceeded a critical cut-off. The prevalence of autism in this sample was 23.5%. Brown, Hobson, Lee, and Stevenson (1997) obtained a similar prevalence rate in their study. They investigated 24 congenitally blind children (aged 3-9 years) without any neurological damage with the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (Schopler, Reichler, & Renner, 1986). They found a prevalence of 20.8% autism.

Hobson et al. (1997) explained their findings by assuming the same underlying abnormal process in autistic children regardless of whether they are blind or sighted. Their explanation based on the "theory of mind," is that there is a lack of intersubjectivity in autistic children, in other words, an inability to understand and to anticipate the thoughts and emotions of others - a lack of shared social understanding. It seems reasonable to expect that children without vision my have difficulties in acquiring intersubjectivity because of their inability to perceive the cues of nonverbal communication. But these difficulties do not necessarily mean a higher risk of autism. Many of the same symptoms in the blind and the sighted child may be explained by divergent processes. The technical term for this phenomenon is "equifinalism," meaning that the same symptoms may be based on different causes.

(1) Different processes. First, the reason for autism in sighted children can be considered to be a pathological information processing. Blind children, in contrast, may have problems in information gathering, but few problems in information processing. Second, whereas the autistic symptoms in sighted children are pervasive problems, they are frequently transient in those who are blind.

(2) Different functions. In addition to different processes, the same so-called autistic symptoms may have different functions in the blind or sighted child.

First, according to arousal theory, stereotypies may have a functional value for children who are blind by reducing stress or uncertainty (Troester, Brambring, & Beelmann, 1994). However, the same stereotypies may be classified as abnormal in the sighted child.

Second, the higher percentage of echolalia may be explained by a different process of language acquisition in children who are blind. Pérez-Pereira and Conti-Ramsden (1999) report empirical data indicating a holistic, gestalt-like language acquisition, based on echolalic, formulaic speech in the first years of life.

Third, the lack of social interaction between blind children and their sighted peers in kindergarten - another symptom for autism in standardized scales - may be explained by two factors: (1) Most kindergarten activities are unsuitable for children who are blind because of the high demands they place on orientation and mobility as well as manual skills (Brambring, 1998). (2) Sighted children of this age have difficulties in anticipating the needs of a peer who is blind (Preisler, 1993). Both of these reasons may explain the social interaction problems in the blind.

Fourth, the last example refers to the frequently observable retardation of symbolic play in children who are blind - another symptom of autism in standardized questionnaires. Once again, this delay may be explained by two different factors: (1) Traditional symbolic toys like dolls or toy automobiles hold hardly any attraction for children who are blind because they offer only a visual representation of the real objects (Troester & Brambring, 1994). (2) Many investigators are unaware of the alternative forms of symbolic play in children who are blind such as verbal role play (Ferguson & Buultjens, 1995). As a result, they may not pick up on the blind-specific forms of symbolic play.

Conclusion

The conclusion that different perspectives need to be taken when explaining autistic symptoms in the blind or sighted population implies a serious warning: Counting only symptoms without taking account of possible differences in underlying processes and functions may lead to false interpretations and intervention strategies for autism in both the sighted and the blind child.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1985). Assessment and taxonomy of child and adolescent psychopathology. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Bauman, M. K. (1964). Group differences disclosed by inventory items. International Journal for the Education of the Blind, 13, 101-106.

Brambring, M. (1998). Lessons with a child who is blind. Development and early intervention in the first years of life. Okemos, MI: Blind Children's Fund.

Brambring, M., & Tröster, H. (in press). Verhaltensauffälligkeiten bei Kindern und Jugendlichen mit Sehschädigung. In P. F. Schlottke, R. K. Silbereisen, S. Schneider, & G. W. Lauth (Ed.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie. Serie II: Klinische Psychologie. Bd. 5: Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

Brown, R. , Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Stevenson, J. (1997). Are there "autistic-like" features in congenitally blind children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 693-703.

Ferguson, R., & Buultjens, M. (1995). The play behaviour of young blind children and its relationship to developmental stages. The British Journal of Visual Impairment, 13, 100-107.

Freeman, R. D., Goetz, D. P., Richards, M., & Groenveld, M. (1991). Defiers of negative prediction: A 14-year follow-up study of legally blind children. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 85, 365-370.

Gelfand, D. M., Jenson, W. R., & Drew, C. J. (1997). Understanding child behaviour disorders. Forth Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.

Goodman, R., & Minne, C. (1995). Questionnaire screening for comorbid pervasive developmental disorders in congenitally blind children: A pilot study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25(2), 195-203.

Hobson, R. P., Brown, R., Minter, M. E., & Lee, A. (1997). "Autism" revisited: The case of congenital blindness. In V. Lewis & G. M. Collis (Eds.), Blindness and psychological development in young children (pp. 99-115). Leicester, England: British Psychological Society Books.

Jan, J. E., Freeman, R. D., & Scott, E. P. (1977). Visual impairment in children and adolescents. New York: Grune & Stratton.

Krug, D. A., Arick, J. R., & Almond, P. J. (1978). ABC - Autism Behaviour Checklist. Portland, OR: ASIEP Education Co.

Pérez-Pereira M., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (1999). Language development and social interaction in blind children. Hove, England: Psychology Press Ltd.

Preisler, G. M. (1993). A descriptive study of blind children in nurseries with sighted children. Child: Care, Health and Development, 19, 295-315.

Rutter, M., Graham, P., & Yule, W. (1970). Neuropsychiatric study in childhood. London: Heinemann.

Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1986). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological.

Tirosh, E., Shnitzer, M. R., Davidovitch, M., & Cohen, A. (1998). Behavioural problems among visually impaired between 6 months and 5 years. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 21, 63- 70.

Tröster, H., & Brambring, M. (1994). The play behaviour and play materials in blind and sighted infants and preschoolers. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 88, 421-432.

Tröster, H., Brambring, M., & Beelmann, A. (1994). Prevalence and situational causes of stereotyped behaviours in blind infants and preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19, 569-590.

World Health Organization (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Michael Brambring, Ph.D.

Department of Psychology

University of Bielefeld

PO 10 01 31

D-33501 Bielefeld, Germany

Phone: ++49-5 21-106-4345/-43 33

Fax : ++49-521-106-8062

e-mail: m.brambring@uni-bielefeld.de

-----------------------

[1]A detailed version with the same title will be published in German in cooperation with Heinrich Troester.

................
................

In order to avoid copyright disputes, this page is only a partial summary.

Google Online Preview   Download