UNC Charlotte Pages
Auditor Narcissism and Auditor-Client NegotiationsBryan K. ChurchGeorgia Institute of TechnologyNarisa Tianjing DaiUniversity of International Business and EconomicsXuejiao LiuUniversity of International Business and EconomicsXi (Jason) KuangGeorgia Institute of TechnologyOctober 2016Please do not quote, cite, or circulate without authors’ permission.Note: The four authors contributed equally to the paper. The authors acknowledge the helpful comments of Lori Bhaskar, Jeff Cohen, Jessen Hobson, Eric Johnson, Jonathan Stanley, Karl Wang, Arnie Wright, and participants at the ABO midyear conference.Auditor Narcissism and Auditor-Client NegotiationsAbstract: This paper reports the results of an experiment designed to examine the effect of auditor narcissism on auditor-client negotiations. We contend that narcissistic characteristics fuel auditors’ competitiveness and embolden them to stand firm in negotiations, potentially curbing clients’ propensity to report opportunistically. Our experiment includes auditor-client dyads who negotiate over a reported asset value. As predicted, we find that narcissistic auditors are more likely to be involved in negotiations that reach an impasse or take longer to resolve, controlling for client narcissism. Importantly, narcissistic auditors negotiate reported asset values that reflect less aggressive reporting choices. Consistent with our experimental results, supplemental analyses using data collected from a naturally-occurring auditor-client setting suggest that auditor narcissism is positively associated with audit delay and negatively associated with clients’ absolute and negative discretionary accruals, again controlling for client narcissism.1. IntroductionThis paper reports the results of an experiment designed to investigate the effect of auditor narcissism on auditor-client negotiations, which ultimately impact the financial-reporting process (Beattie et al. 2000; Gibbins et al. 2001). Narcissism is a personality trait that involves a grandiose preoccupation with one’s superiority and self-importance (American Psychiatric Association 2013), with people existing along a continuum from low to high (Foster and Campbell 2007; Grijalva and Harms 2014). We posit that auditor narcissism can have a pronounced effect on negotiations over accounting disagreements, helping auditors curtail managers’ opportunistic reporting behavior. Extant evidence suggests that corporate executives often possess narcissistic characteristics (Chatterjee and Hambrick 2007; Nevicka et al. 2011), underlying aggressive reporting choices (Hales et al. 2012; Ham et al. 2015; Judd et al. 2015; Majors 2015). For example, more narcissistic CFOs are associated with less timely loss recognition, higher levels of accruals-based and real earnings management, and a greater chance of accounting restatements (Ham et al. 2015). Relatedly, auditors anticipate greater challenges when dealing with executives who exhibit narcissistic characteristics (e.g., grandiosity, entitlement, and exploitativeness). Under such conditions, auditors’ assessment of fraud risk is heightened (Johnson et al. 2013), audit fees are increased (reflecting greater potential exposure), and the chance of auditors resigning is elevated (Judd et al. 2015). We extend this line of study by examining auditor narcissism as a means to bolster audit quality.We use an experimental-economics approach to investigate the effect of auditor narcissism on the financial-reporting process. We create auditor-client dyads, whereby paired participants negotiate over reported amounts. Prior studies suggest that financial statements are a joint product determined by auditor-client interactions (Antle and Nalebuff 1991; Beattie et al. 2000; Salterio 2012), such that negotiations are critical in establishing reported amounts (e.g., resolving disagreements over the application of accounting standards, estimating uncertain amounts, or deciding whether to book or waive misstatements). We offer insight into how auditor narcissism affects auditor-client negotiations.Accounting researchers have shown considerable interest recently in auditors’ personality traits (Scofield et al. 2004; Hurtt 2010; Quadackers et al. 2014; Bauer 2015; Bhaskar et al. 2015; Church et al. 2015; Fitzgerald et al. 2015). Akers et al. (2014) find that accounting partners, on average, have the highest narcissism score across public accountants’ professional ranks. Furthermore, partners’ average scores are highest on certain dimensions, including authority, entitlement, and self-sufficiency (not needing others). We put forth that partners’ narcissism can have a marked effect on auditor-client negotiations, influencing the negotiation process and potentially bettering audit quality. Partners’ narcissism is expected to impact auditor-client negotiations over accounting and reporting matters. Narcissists are self-absorbed and unyielding in negotiations, failing to see the merits of their counterparts’ claims (Greenhalgh and Gilkey 1997). Narcissists lack empathy (Watson et al. 1984), making it difficult for them to understand the expectations, viewpoints, motivations, and emotions of their counterparts (Greenhalgh and Gilkey 1997; Park et al. 2013). Hence, narcissists are less agreeable in situations involving interpersonal conflict and even antagonistic in negotiations (Miller and Campbell 2008; Grijalva and Harms 2014). Auditors’ narcissism causes them to be dogged and uncompromising, refusing to condone clients’ aggressive reporting choices. Such behavior can lengthen the negotiation process, forestalling agreements, and in some instances bring about an impasse. Indeed, auditor narcissism can lead to audit delay. Though such an outcome is undesirable, auditors may have little alternative, beyond succumbing to clients’ demands. Narcissists are driven to come out on top, taking whatever actions are necessary to win (Ryckman et al. 1994; Watson et al. 1997–1998; Luchner et al. 2011). They are ultracompetitive, emboldened to stand firm in negotiations. Auditors’ narcissism propels them to hold their ground on matters involving accounting disagreements (Ma and Jaeger 2005; Hüffmeier et al. 2014). They are prone to resist clients’ efforts to report opportunistically. Indeed, partners’ narcissism can serve to counter the effects of executives’ narcissism, which have been associated with clients’ earnings management and accounting restatements (Ham et al. 2015; Judd et al. 2015). The end result is that, when disagreements are resolved, reported amounts are less aggressive.We are primarily interested in auditor narcissism and how its shapes reported amounts, which reflect a joint auditor-client outcome. The effect of auditor narcissism, however, is subject to two important contextual influences: bargaining power and client narcissism. In our experiment, we create a setting in which the auditor and client have equal bargaining power, precluding either party from having an advantage in negotiations. Equal bargaining power provides an important baseline, which can be used to gauge auditor-client negotiations. For client narcissism, we measure this variable and include it in our data analyses.We conduct an experimental-economics study using undergraduate business students at a major university in China. Prior to conducting our experiment, we administer the 40-item, forced-choice narcissistic personality inventory (NPI), a commonly used self-report measure of narcissism in normal populations (Raskin and Terry 1988; Miller et al. 2014). Based on participants’ NPI scores, we assign them to auditor-client dyads and have them negotiate over a series of periods. We examine the linkage between narcissism and the negotiation process and outcomes. Our primary findings indicate that, as predicted, narcissistic auditors are more likely to be involved in protracted negotiations and, further, to agree on reported amounts that are less aggressive. To gain further insight into the effect of auditor narcissism on the financial-reporting process, we perform supplemental analyses using archival data on publicly listed companies in China. For these companies, audit reports are signed by the auditor, which is noteworthy because signature size has been used by others as a proxy for narcissism (Ham et al. 2014; Ham et al. 2015). Thus, we are able to measure auditor narcissism in a naturally-occurring, auditor-client setting. We also obtain the CFO’s signature from the annual report, allowing us to control for client narcissism. We find that auditor narcissism is positively associated with audit delay, which is consistent with negotiations being lengthy (e.g., Salterio 2012). In addition, auditor narcissism is negatively associated with clients’ signed, positive discretionary accruals and clients’ absolute discretionary accruals, suggestive of less aggressive reporting choices. The supplemental results are consistent with our experimental findings. Importantly, the use of multiple research methods helps overcome the potential drawbacks inherent in each method, lending credence to our theory and enhancing the external validity of our findings (Smith 1975; Jick 1979; Scandura and Williams 2000). Researchers have long advocated the use of a multi-method approach, as it supports triangulation and instills confidence in convergent findings (e.g., Einhorn and Hogarth 1981; Birnberg et al. 1990; Brewer and Hunter 2006; Hageman 2008).Our findings contribute to a growing accounting literature, which suggests that auditors’ personality traits impact audit quality. For example, Quadackers et al. (2014) show that auditors who exhibit lower levels of interpersonal trust make more skeptical judgments in high-risk situations, potentially for the betterment of audit quality. Church et al. (2015) find that dispositional perspective taking enhances auditors’ assessment of clients’ earnings, promoting higher quality financial reporting. We add to this literature by showing that narcissism affects the outcome of auditor-client negotiations, dampening clients’ attempts to report aggressively.Our findings also have significant practical implications. Employees’ attributes (e.g., personality traits) have become increasingly important in accounting firms’ recruiting efforts and promotion decisions (Campbell 2012). Our findings highlight the potential effects of auditor narcissism on the audit process and outputs. Accounting firms may need to consider these effects to enhance audit efficiencies and audit quality by assigning the “right” personnel to specific jobs. For example, audit partners who are more narcissistic may be better able to cope with contentious clients. The remainder of this paper is organized as follows. Section 2 offers a framework for our study and provides a basis to develop the research hypotheses. Section 3 describes our experiment, including participants, design, and procedures. Section 4 presents the experimental results and discusses the findings. Section 5 reports the results of additional archival analyses, which supplement our experimental findings. Section 6 summarizes our study and offers concluding remarks.2. FrameworkAuditor-client interactions occur on an ongoing basis throughout the audit process. Disagreements give rise to conflict, which provides the basis for negotiations (e.g., Greenhalgh 1987; McCracken et al. 2008). Importantly, audited financial statements represent a negotiated outcome; a joint statement of the auditor and manager (Antle and Nalebuff 1991). Beattie et al. (2000) survey finance directors and audit partners and provide evidence that negotiations frequently involve subjective accounting matters (e.g., fair value assessments in acquisitions). Gibbins et al. (2007) survey CFOs and document that negotiations entail divergent preferences, new and unique accounting issues, and complex accounting matters. McCracken et al. (2008) interview CFOs and audit partners and find that accounting standards and interpretations often are invoked in negotiations. As such, auditor-client negotiations can have a material effect on financial statements (Beattie et al. 2000; Gibbins et al. 2001). Below, we consider how auditor narcissism affects the negotiation process and negotiated outcomes. 2.1. Narcissism and the Length of NegotiationsAuditors’ narcissistic characteristics can impact the negotiation process, potentially delaying agreement on reported amounts and, at the extreme, leading to a stalemate or impasse. A narcissistic personality entails a heightened self-image, including a sense of dominance, superiority, and grandiosity (Emmons 1984, 1987; Raskin and Terry 1988; Kubarych et al. 2004). This facet of narcissism is linked to arrogance, self-importance, self-confidence, and assuredness. It is associated with an undue focus on self (Emmons 1987) and a need for power (Carroll 1987), manifesting in self-aggrandizing behavior (Morf and Rhodewalt 2001). Kong (2015) finds that negotiators’ narcissism is negatively associated with their assessments of counterparts’ competence, indicating that counterparts are viewed as inferior. Narcissists are inclined to disparage others who doubt or challenge them. Rhodewalt and Morf (1995) suggest that, in the face of interpersonal conflict, narcissists react with cynical hostility, antagonism, and combativeness. Simply put, narcissists are more likely to be disagreeable in negotiations than others (Miller and Campbell 2008). Narcissists’ self-aggrandizement makes it more difficult to reach an agreement in negotiations, thereby slowing the process.A narcissistic personality also encompasses individuals’ sense of entitlement and a lack of regard for others (Emmons 1984, 1987; Watson et al. 1984; Raskin and Terry 1988). A feeling of entitlement is indicative of believing that one deserves more than others, and being willing to exploit others for selfish reasons. Grijalva and Harms (2014) suggest that narcissists approach negotiations with an individualistic orientation: they focus primarily on personal gain, with little concern for how their actions affect others (Campbell et al. 2005). Such an approach can delay agreement (De Dreu et al. 2000) and, in some instances, produce an impasse (Babcock and Loewenstein 2005). Lastly, narcissists lack cognitive and emotional empathy, making it hard for them to understand others’ viewpoints and feelings (Watson et al. 1984; Watson et al. 1992). Hepper et al. (2014) find that young male adults currently serving a prison sentence have higher levels of narcissism, particularly entitlement, than those without a criminal history and, further, that the relationship is mediated by low levels of cognitive and emotional empathy. Narcissists’ insensitivity to others can hinder negotiations, extending the process. Based on the preceding discussion, we posit that auditor narcissism impacts the time needed to resolve disagreements over reported amounts. Our first research hypothesis is formally stated as follows.H1: Auditor narcissism is positively associated with the length of the negotiation process.2.2. Narcissism and Negotiation OutcomesResearch on auditor-client negotiations suggests that disagreements generally are resolved, resulting in an unqualified audit opinion, with the auditor being reappointed (Gibbins et al. 2001). A common negotiation outcome is an agreement between the two parties’ original positions, though in some instances the outcome is in line with one party’s initial stance (Gibbins et al. 2001; Gibbins et al. 2007). Negotiated outcomes tend to be distributive, meaning that one or neither party wins (Gibbins et al. 2001; Gibbins et al. 2005, 2007; Brown and Wright 2008; Brown and Johnstone 2009). We maintain that auditor narcissism affects the auditor’s need to win and, thus, has a bearing on negotiation outcomes, impacting whether reported amounts are less aggressive (as preferred by the auditor), holding client narcissism constant.Narcissists possess characteristics that drive them to stand their ground in negotiations. Narcissists are described as self-absorbed and self-serving (Greenhalgh and Gilkey 1997; Ronningstam 2009). Such characteristics compel them to exert their superiority and dominance in negotiations, whereby others eventually accede to their demands. Indeed, prior findings document that narcissism is positively associated with individuals’ competitiveness (i.e., their desire to win in interpersonal settings) and hypercompetitiveness (i.e., their need to win at all costs in order to feel superior) (Ryckman et al. 1994; Watson et al. 1997–1998; Luchner et al. 2011). Park et al. (2013) conduct a simulated one-shot negotiation with MBA students and find that narcissists generate greater economic gains than non-narcissists; however, narcissists also incur greater interpersonal loss (i.e., they are judged to be less trustworthy than non-narcissists). Relatedly, Campbell et al. (2005) conduct an experiment that mimics the tragedy of the commons dilemma, with participants making decisions over a series of periods. The study’s findings indicate that narcissists are more likely to realize short-term success, benefitting self, but at a long-term cost to others.Narcissism also is linked to individuals’ assertiveness, aggressiveness, antagonism, and intransigence (Baumeister et al. 2000; Hopwood et al. 2013; Küfner et al. 2013; Furnham and Crump 2014; Jones and Neria 2015). These characteristics underscore narcissists’ competitiveness in negotiations and manifest in the use of a contending strategy (Sandy et al. 2006), whereby individuals focus on achieving a specific outcome with little regard for their counterparts’ welfare (Wang and Tuttle 2009). A contending strategy involves demanding concessions from one’s counterparts, while at the same time resisting offers and counteroffers. Contending tactics include asserting positional commitments, arguing forcefully and persuasively, making threats, and giving ultimatums (Carnevale and Pruitt 1992). Previous research suggests that auditors use contending tactics when clients are inflexible on a disputed accounting position (Gibbins et al. 2010; Hollindale et al. 2011). In such cases, auditors put forth arguments to substantiate their positions and also to refute clients’ positions (Weingart et al. 1996; Hyder et al. 2000). Auditors also can threaten to modify their audit opinions in order to coerce concessions from clients (Bame-Aldred and Kida 2007). For our purposes, narcissists’ use of contending tactics, such as being intransigent and unyielding, can provide a means to achieve a desired outcome (e.g., Barry and Friedman 1998). Ma and Jaeger (2005) conduct simulated negotiations with undergraduates and find that aggressive and combative tactics lead to outcomes that are consistent with negotiators’ preferences. Hüffmeier et al. (2014) perform a meta-analysis of 34 studies to examine the effect of bargaining strategy on negotiation outcomes. The study’s findings indicate that hardline bargaining (i.e., making extreme initial offers and minimizing own concessions) results in larger economic outcomes (i.e., hardline bargainers do better than others in negotiations). In fact, hardline bargaining may even be fitness-enhancing from an evolutionary perspective (Heifetz and Segev 2004).We contend that auditor narcissism is associated with an inherent drive to prevail in negotiations with clients. The drive to win is underscored by a focus on self, providing an avenue to exert one’s dominance and superiority. Further, the drive to win triggers competitive behaviors, including steadfast and uncompromising negotiating tactics. Notably, Trotman et al. (2009) provide evidence that audit partners consider tough and conservative stances when contemplating inventory write-downs as part of pre-negotiation planning (i.e., expectations that partners bring to negotiations). We argue that narcissistic auditors are less inclined to move away from such hardline positions, even though the positions are at odds with client preferences. In turn, narcissist auditors are more likely to get their way in negotiations than others, demanding that clients opt for less aggressive reporting choices. Based on the preceding discussion, our second research hypothesis is formally stated as follows. H2: Auditor narcissism is positively associated with negotiation outcomes that reflect less aggressive reported amounts. As mentioned previously, in this study we focus on the effect of auditor narcissism on the negotiation process and outcome. The way in which narcissistic characteristics influence negotiators’ behavior, however, is not specific to the role of the negotiator. Our arguments underlying the effect of auditor narcissism on negotiations could just as easily be applied to client narcissism. That is, ceteris paribus, narcissistic clients could be more likely to have protracted negotiations and obtain a negotiated outcome in their favor (e.g., more aggressive reporting choices), as compared to non-narcissists. In our hypotheses tests, we control for client narcissism and also report the effect of client narcissism on negotiations.3. Research Method3.1. Participants and DesignWe design a negotiation experiment in which we manipulate the composition of auditor-client dyads based on participants’ predisposition to engage in narcissistic behavior. Approximately two weeks before conducting the experiment, we administer the 40-item, forced choice narcissistic personality inventory (NPI) to 662 undergraduate business students at a major Chinese university (Raskin and Terry 1988). The NPI is a standard measure of non-pathological (nonclinical) narcissism, and it has been validated using undergraduate students in China. Kwan et al. (2009) document that the responses of Chinese undergraduates are correlated with key features of narcissism, self-esteem, and self-efficacy, and the correlations are similar to those reported in relevant research using U.S. undergraduates. The range of possible NPI scores is 0 – 40, with higher scores representing higher degrees of narcissism. In a sample of 3,445 adults worldwide, Foster et al. (2003) report an average NPI score of 15.2, and the average is similar (15.3) restricting the sample to respondents who reside in the U.S. (n = 2,546). Akers et al. (2014) administer the NPI to practicing accountants in the Midwest U.S. (n = 279) and find that the average score is 14.7. The average is slightly higher (15.3) including only those in auditing (n = 155). In the current study, the average NPI of the 662 Chinese undergraduates is 13.7. We suggest that individuals’ responses to the NPI in China are comparable to those in the U.S. We partition our sample into terciles based on their NPI score. We recruit from the top and bottom third for our experiment, representing High (denoted H) and Low (denoted L) narcissists, respectively. One hundred ninety-six students agree to participate in our negotiation experiment. The high narcissists who take part have NPI scores ranging from 17 to 34, with an average of 22.5 (n = 99). The low narcissists, on the other hand, have NPI scores ranging from 0 to 9, with an average of 6.0 (n = 97). Our experimental participants have an average age of 20.3 years and 75 percent are female. In our experiment, one half of the participants are assigned the role of auditor and the other half client. We create auditor-client dyads that negotiate over reported amounts. The experiment consists of a series of period, with participants re-paired each period. Auditor-client incentives are conflicting (described later), providing a basis for negotiations. We configure the composition of the dyads such that both participants are H, both are L, or one is H and one L. We have four experimental groups denoted HH, LL, HL, and LH, where the first letter represents the auditor-participant’s narcissism and the second letter the client-participant’s narcissism. We systematically configure the dyads, as described above, for two reasons. First, the narcissism of each role (auditor-client) is held constant over the course of an experimental session. Second, although our focus is on auditor narcissism, client narcissism also can impact the negotiation process and negotiated outcomes. For example, prior research suggests that narcissistic clients report aggressively (Ham et al. 2015; Judd et al. 2015). Hence, we control for client narcissism in establishing auditor-client dyads. In addition, we include client narcissism in our data analyses and present relevant results.3.2. Experimental ProceduresThe experiment is administered in a university computer lab. Based on students’ availability, we conduct 12 experimental sessions, with three sessions per experimental group. The number of participants (dyads) per session ranges from 12 to 20 (6 to 10). Overall, we have 25, 24, 22, and 27 dyads for the HH, LL, HL, and LH groups, respectively. Once participants arrive, instructions are distributed and read aloud by one of the researchers. The instructions describe participant pairings, the negotiation setting and protocol, and participants’ payoffs. Subsequently, participants answer quiz questions covering the instructions. The quiz is included to ensure that participants understand the instructions, and participants are not allowed to proceed until all questions are answered correctly.Experimental sessions consist of 18 independent, one-minute periods. Participants are not informed beforehand of the number of periods, as a means to avoid potential end-game effects. Each period auditor-client dyads negotiate over a reported amount, which represents asset value. Negotiations take place over the computer and are anonymous. The negotiation setting is programmed using the Z-tree software (Fischbacher 2007). We pair participants anonymously in order to control for potentially confounding effects, including participants appearance, gender, mannerisms, and prior acquaintance (i.e., whether paired participants know one another). At period end, auditor-client dyads are randomly reassigned. Participants do not receive feedback on realized asset values throughout the experiment. We suppress feedback in order to reinforce the fact that each period represents an independent, one-shot negotiation. Furthermore, feedback on asset value is lagged in naturally-occurring settings. Our experimental context is purposefully stark, which circumvents possible confounds associated with participants’ beliefs about how they are supposed to behave in auditor-client settings. We are interested in how auditor-client dyads determine a reported amount, abstracting away from other potentially noisy factors (see also Wang and Tuttle 2009; Wang 2010).At the beginning of each period, neither player knows the actual asset value. We make this choice to control participants’ bargaining power, such that one player does not have an advantage over the other (discussed below). The players negotiate over an acceptable reported asset value, which impacts their payoffs. The actual asset value is determined by drawing from a uniform distribution that ranges from 50 to 1,000 in increments of 50. So the asset takes one of 20 values, each having a 5 percent chance of realization. The actual asset value is randomly generated for each auditor-client dyad per period. The instructions include a chart that lists potential values, the probability of occurrence, and the probability that the amount exceeds the actual asset value (see also Wang and Tuttle 2009; Wang 2010). The chart is reproduced in Table 1. [Insert Table 1 about here.]Negotiations take place as follows. The client begins by proposing a reported amount. The reported amount is communicated to the auditor, who either accepts or makes a counteroffer. If the auditor accepts, the period ends. If the auditor makes a counteroffer, the client either accepts or proposes another amount. The process continues back and forth until an amount is accepted or time expires. As mentioned earlier, periods can last up to one minute. We deem the allotted time sufficient because the setting is stark and straightforward. Further, the experimental data suggest that the time is adequate because, on average, we observe more than 13 offers per period before an agreement is reached, and agreements are achieved 76 percent of the time.Players’ payoffs are conflicting such that the client prefers larger reported amounts and the auditor prefers amounts that do not exceed the actual asset value. In addition, players’ payoffs are contingent on whether an agreement is reached: if an impasse occurs, both players’ payoffs are zero. If an agreement is reached, the client’s payoff is the reported amount. By comparison, the auditor’s payoff depends on the relation between the reported amount and the actual asset value. The auditor’s payoff is 400 if the reported amount is equal to or less than the actual value. Otherwise, the auditor’s payoff is 400 minus a penalty that is computed as one half of the difference between the reported amount and the actual asset value. In other words, the auditor incurs a penalty for allowing a reported amount that is too aggressive, where the penalty is increasing in the level of aggressiveness. The penalty encompasses litigation exposure and regulatory pressure, which are relevant and present in naturally-occurring auditor-client settings. The potential for a penalty means that the auditor prefers smaller reported amounts, which is consistent with the audit ecology. Players’ payoffs are summarized in Figure 1. [Insert Figure 1 about here.]The negotiation setting is structured so that both player roles have equal bargaining power (i.e., neither party has an advantage over the other). The balance of power in auditor-client negotiations is contingent on situational factors (Gibbins et al. 2001; Brown and Wright 2008; Salterio 2012). For example, the auditor’s relative power is fortified by a strong audit committee and authoritative accounting standards (supporting the auditor’s position). By comparison, the client’s relative power is strengthened by the importance of the client to the auditor and firsthand knowledge of the negotiation issue. Our setting allows us to study the effect of auditor narcissism on auditor-client negotiations, holding bargaining power constant and equal across the two player roles.After finishing 18 periods, participants complete a post-experiment questionnaire, which collects demographics as well as other information about participants’ perceptions and assessments of the experiment. Participants’ cash payouts for taking part in the experiment are determined as follows. For each session, we rank client-participants and auditor-participants based on their experimental earnings and pay them a fixed amount: the participant who is ranked first in each role makes the most, the participant who is ranked second makes the next most, and so forth. The procedure allows us to create similar incentives for participants in different player roles (Wang and Tuttle 2009; Wang 2010). Further, it eliminates participants’ tendency to compare own payoffs with their counterpart’s payoffs (Bolton 1991). The procedure is appropriate because we are not concerned with auditor versus client payoffs, rather we are interested in the effect of auditor narcissism on the negotiation process and outcome.4. ResultsTable 2 presents descriptive statistics for various measures representing auditor-client negotiations. The data are collapsed across periods to provide simple summary measures. The descriptive statistics are arranged to allow for visual comparisons of auditor-client pairings, representing the four experimental groups. We tabulate the percentage of time that an agreement is reached (AGREE), the average number of offers before agreeing (OFFERS), and the average agreed amount (VALUE). We use data from all 18 dyads/periods to compute AGREE and data from dyads/periods with an agreement to compute OFFERS and VALUE. [Insert Table 2 about here.]Two measures, AGREE and OFFERS, represent the duration of the negotiation process. The process is protracted to the extent that AGREE decreases (i.e., impasses occur) and OFFERS increases. Our first hypothesis suggests that high narcissist auditors (H) are more likely to be involved in prolonged negotiations than low narcissist auditors (L). The other measure, VALUE, represents the outcome of the negotiation process, conditioned on agreement. The auditor prefers agreements to the extent that VALUE decreases, reflecting less aggressive reported amounts. Our second hypothesis suggests that H auditors are more likely to agree on smaller reported amounts in negotiations than L auditors. Specific predictions are summarized in Table 3. [Insert Table 3 about here.]As discussed earlier, our arguments regarding the effect of auditor narcissism on negotiations also can apply to client narcissism, particularly when the two parties have equal bargaining power. Below, we report the effects of auditor narcissism as well as client narcissism on the negotiation process and outcome.4.1. Tests of H1To test H1 we perform analyses focusing on the duration of the negotiation process: AGREE and OFFERS. For AGREE, the dependent variable is coded as 1 if an agreement is reached and 0 otherwise. The independent variables include auditor-participant’s narcissism (AuditorN), client-participants’ narcissism (ClientN), an interaction term (AuditorN x ClientN), and the client’s initial offer (1st?Offer). AuditorN and ClientN are dummy variables, coded as 1 for High (H) and 0 for Low (L). We control for 1st?Offer because it establishes a starting point and potentially impacts the negotiation process (Chertkoff and Conley 1967; Galinsky and Mussweiler 2001). Because AGREE is a categorical variable, we perform a logistic regression.Recall that in our experiment, players are re-paired to form a different negotiating dyad each period. Unless otherwise specified, our main analyses reported in this subsection treat each negotiating dyad as an independent data point. For analyses where the data can be broken down to the individual player level, we repeat the test using each player as a data point, controlling for repeated measurement by including the player as a cluster variable, and statistical inferences are unchanged. The logistic regression results are summarized in Panel A of Table 4. We find that the coefficient of AuditorN is negative (β = ?0.841) and statistically significant at p < 0.001. The negative coefficient indicates that H auditors are less likely to reach an agreement (more likely to reach an impasse) than L auditors, which is consistent with H1. We also find that the coefficient of ClientN is negative (β = ?0.479) and statistically significant at p = 0.007, paralleling the effect of AuditorN. Notably, the interaction term (AuditorN x ClientN) is not significant (p > 0.40). Hence, our results suggest that an agreement is less likely when at least one of the negotiating parties (auditor or client) is a high narcissist. The cell means, summarized in Table 2, indicate that the frequency of agreement is highest when both negotiating parties are low narcissists, with the percentages decreasing when the auditor and/or client is a high narcissist. Finally, the coefficient of 1st?Offer is negative (β = ?0.002) and statistically significant at p < 0.001, indicating that the failure to reach an agreement is associated with higher initial offers. Recall that clients prefer higher values, while auditors prefer lower values. Our findings provide evidence that more extreme starting points (i.e., initial offers that promote clients’ self-interests) can result in an impasse.[Insert Table 4 about here.]Next, we perform an analysis using OFFERS as the dependent variable, representing the total number of offers preceding an agreement. The independent variables are the same as those described above, although the observations are restricted to agreements. We perform an ordinary least squares (OLS) regression, with the results summarized in Panel A of Table 4. The coefficient of AuditorN is positive (β = 3.975) and statistically significant at p < 0.01, which indicates more offers with H auditors than L auditors. We maintain that increasing the number of offers extends the negotiation process. As might be expected, we document a statistically significant correlation between OFFERS and the time taken to reach an agreement (ρ = 0.50, p = 0.0001). Our findings suggest that negotiations are more likely to be drawn out with H auditors as compared to L auditors, providing further support for H1. The OLS findings also indicate that the coefficient of ClientN is positive (β = 2.658) and statistically significant at p = 0.008, again paralleling the effect of AuditorN. Further, 1st?Offer and the interaction term are both statistically significant at p < 0.025. For 1st–Offer, the coefficient is positive (β = 0.013), indicating that higher first offers prolong the negotiation process (i.e., more offers before reaching an agreement). To interpret the statistically significant interaction term, we partition the data by client narcissism and examine the simple effects. The simple effects results are summarized in Panel B of Table 4. In both analyses, the coefficient of AuditorN is positive and statistically significant (p < 0.05), though the effect is more pronounced with L clients than H clients. Scrutinizing the data by experimental condition, we note that the difference in OFFERS is greater when comparisons involve the LL dyad (i.e., when both parties are low narcissists). With the LL dyad, the number of offers preceding an agreement is noticeably lower than in other dyads.All in all, our findings support H1. Auditor narcissism is associated with the likelihood of reaching an impasse in negotiations. In addition, auditor narcissism is associated with the length of the negotiation process, and this relationship is more discernible when auditors negotiate with clients who are low, rather than high, on the narcissistic personality dimension. 4.2. Tests of H2To test H2, we examine the agreed amount, which represents the negotiated outcome. We perform an OLS regression using VALUE as the dependent measure. The independent variables are the same as those described earlier. The OLS findings, summarized in Panel A of Table 4 (rightmost column), indicate that the coefficient of AuditorN is negative (β = ?28.837) and statistically significant at p < 0.01. The negative coefficient indicates that the agreed amount is smaller (less aggressive) with H auditors than L auditors, supporting H2. We also find that the coefficient of ClientN is positive (β = 42.978) and statistically significant (p < 0.01). In this case, the agreed amount is larger (more aggressive) with H clients than L clients. An inspection of the cell means, summarized in Table 2, sheds further light on the effects of auditor and client narcissism on the negotiated outcome. When the dyad includes a high and low narcissist (HL and LH dyads), the agreed amount is more in line with the high narcissist’s preferences: that is, the agreed amount is lower with H auditors and higher with H clients. When the dyad includes two high narcissists (HH dyad), the agreed amount falls between that of the HL and LH dyads. In this case, players’ narcissistic behavior appears to offset one another. The auditor’s narcissism works to dampen the reported amount, and the client’s narcissism works to increase the amount. Lastly, when the dyad includes two low narcissists (LL dyad), the agreed amount is similar to that of the HH dyad. Both dyads (LL and HH) end up agreeing on a reported amount that seems to reflect a middle ground; however, the negotiation process differs markedly. More specifically, the LL dyad reaches more agreements and makes fewer offers than the HH dyad (refer to Table 2).The OLS results also indicate that the coefficient of 1st?Offer is positive (β = 0.268) and statistically significant (p < 0.01). Higher initial offers are associated with higher agreed amounts, which is consistent with prior findings in the negotiation literature (Chertkoff and Conley 1967; Galinsky and Mussweiler 2001). As discussed earlier though, higher initial offers can lead to protracted negotiations: that is, fewer agreements and more offers before reaching an agreement.Overall, our findings are consistent with H2. Auditors who score high, rather than low, on the narcissistic personality inventory are more likely to agree on an amount that is smaller (less aggressive) and, thus, more in line with their preferences. In our experimental setting, auditors’ prefer a smaller reported amount because it reduces the chance of incurring a penalty (e.g., costs associated with legal liability and dealing with regulators). We also document that client narcissism is associated with the agreed amount, but in the opposite direction. That is, clients who score high, rather than low, on the narcissistic personality inventory are more likely to agree on an amount that is higher (more aggressive), which is more in line with their preferences. In sum, participants’ narcissism has a very marked effect on the negotiation outcome. 5. Supplementary Data and ResultsTo ensure internal validity, we use a stylized negotiation setting in our experiment. The setting abstracts away from professional/ethical values (e.g., conforming to a code of conduct/ethics), social costs (auditor-client relationships), and institutional knowledge (e.g., experience/expertise surrounding the application of GAAP). The setting also does not include certain features common to negotiation settings, such as persuasive communication, threats, opportunities for reputation building, and ongoing relationships. We believe that our design choices are necessary to ensure a clean and parsimonious test of our hypotheses. To shed light on the applicability of our findings to naturally-occurring auditor-client settings, we collect archival data and perform additional analyses. We collect data related to issues that correspond to our hypotheses. Our first hypothesis suggests that auditor narcissism is associated with protracted negotiations. For this hypothesis we focus on audit delay, defined as the length of time from fiscal year end to the audit report date (Ashton et al. 1987). Salterio (2012) points out that auditor-client negotiations likely play a role in audit delay. For example, disagreements over the application of GAAP, particularly late in the process, hamper the completion of an engagement. We use audit delay as a proxy for the time associated with auditor-client negotiations. Our second hypothesis suggests that auditor narcissism results in reported amounts that are less aggressive. We use absolute and signed discretionary accruals as a proxy for negotiated outcomes. We assume that the auditor prefers smaller amounts, as it constrains the client’s latitude in financial reporting. Prior findings suggest that clients’ discretionary accruals are associated with auditors’ litigation risk: that is, higher discretionary accruals increase the likelihood of auditor liability (e.g., Boone et al. 2011). 5.1. Auditor NarcissismWe collect archival data on Chinese public companies, which provides a natural basis of comparison with our experimental data, also collected in China. Importantly, the auditor’s report in China is signed by two auditors, a review auditor and an engagement auditor. The review auditor typically is the lead auditor, having ultimate authority and responsibility (Chinese Ministry of Finance 2001; Liu and Chi 2014). The engagement auditor, on the other hand, directs the audit (Wang et al. 2015). The review auditor usually is more senior and experienced than the engagement auditor (Gul et al. 2013; Lennox et al. 2014). The signature of the review auditor is placed above that of the engagement auditor, which allows us to identify the two roles (Lennox et al. 2014; Wang et al. 2015). We measure the size of each signature following prior research (Zweigenhaft 1977; Ham et al. 2014; Ham et al. 2015). Specifically, we construct a rectangle around each signature, with each side touching the most extreme point of the signature. Then, we standardize the area, dividing by the number of characters in the signature. This approach yields the area-per-letter, which helps to control for differences in name lengths. Prior research provides evidence of a positive association between signature size and various facets of personality linked to narcissism, including awareness of status (Zweigenhaft 1970; Zweigenhaft and Marlow 1973), self-esteem (Zweigenhaft 1977), and dominance (Jorgenson 1977). Previous accounting research suggests a positive relation between signature size and narcissism: that is, narcissists have larger signatures than others (Ham et al. 2014; Ham et al. 2015). We take additional steps to validate signature size as a proxy for narcissism. We recruit 52 undergraduate business students at a major Chinese university and administer the 40-item NPI (Raskin and Terry 1988). We also collect students’ signatures. The standardized signature size is positively correlated with students’ NPI score (ρ = 0.27, p = 0.053, two-tailed). The correlation is similar to that reported in Ham et al. (2015), and offers evidence of a linkage between signature size and narcissism. 5.2. SampleOur sample includes 429 Chinese A-share listed firm-year observations from 2007 – 2013, representing 281 firms in 53 industries. The shares are listed on the Shanghai and Shenzhen Stock Exchanges, the two major exchanges in China. We begin our sample period in 2007, which corresponds with a significant change in China’s financial reporting regime. In 2006, the Ministry of Finance of China issued new accounting standards for business enterprises, effective January 1, 2007.5.3. Audit DelayFirst, we estimate the following OLS regression model to examine the effect of auditor narcissism on audit delay.LnLagit=α0+α1SIZEAUD1j+α2SIZEAUD2j+α3SIZECFOj+α4BIGit+α5SIZEit+α6GROWTHRit+α7LEVit+α8QUICKit+α9INVRECit+α10LOSSit+α11FOREIGNit+α12FIRMAGEit+α13BOARDINDit+ α14BOARDMEETit+α15TOPSHit+α16-21YearIndicators+IndustryIndicators+Error.The dependent variable, LnLagit, is the natural log of the number of days between a company’s fiscal year-end and the audit report date. The natural log transformation typically is applied when the variable is positively skewed, as with audit delay. For comparative purposes, we also perform the analysis using SLagit, defined as the number of days between fiscal year-end and the audit report date scaled by 360 days. We point out that all publicly listed companies in China have a December 31st fiscal year-end, and are required to disclose an annual report no later than April 30th. The primary independent variables of interest are SIZEAUD1 and SIZEAUD2. These variables proxy for auditor narcissism based on the signature size of the review auditor and engagement auditor, respectively. We include SIZECFO to proxy for the client’s narcissism based on the CFO’s signature size. The CFO usually takes part in auditor-client negotiations and, thus, can impact the duration of the process. Gibbins et al. (2001) report that CFOs are involved in 96 percent of the negotiation examples identified by audit partners. In another study, Gibbins et al. (2007) report that CFOs are involved 81 percent of the time. In addition, we interviewed several audit partners in China who confirmed that the CFO typically is involved in auditor-client negotiations.We include numerous control variables in our analysis based on prior research (e.g., Ashton et al. 1989; Ettredge et al. 2006; Whitworth and Lambert 2014). We expect auditor and client characteristics to impact the negotiation process and, in turn, audit delay. The variables are defined in Table 5 and summarized below. For the auditor, we include an indicator variable for Big 4 auditor (BIG). For client characteristics, we include the natural log of total assets (SIZE), growth in revenues (GROWTHR), the leverage ratio (LEV), the quick ratio (QUICK), inventory and receivables divided by total assets (INVREC), negative earnings (LOSS), foreign operations (FOREIGN), the natural log of firm age (FIRMAGE), the percentage of independent board directors (BOARDIND), the number of board meetings (BOARDMEET), and the percentage of shares held by the top ten shareholders (TOPSH). In addition, we control for year (YearIndicators) and industry (IndustryIndicators) fixed effects, and we adjust standard errors for clustering at the firm level. All continuous variables also are winsorized at the top and bottom one percent to avoid the impact of outliers. [Insert Table 5 about here.]Table 6 reports descriptive statistics for audit delay (LnLag and SLag), auditor and client narcissism (SIZEAUD1, SIZEAUD2, and SIZECFO), and other control variables. We observe that audit delay is, on average, 91.4 days. Inspection of Table 6 indicates that SIZEAUD1 is larger than SIZEAUD2, suggesting that the review auditor’s narcissism exceeds that of the engagement auditor. Moreover, SIZEAUD1 and SIZEAUD2 are larger than SIZECFO. This difference, however, is unlikely to be related to narcissism because, in general, more space is allotted for signatures in the auditor’s report than in the annual report. [Insert Table 6 about here.]The left-hand side of Table 7 presents the results of two separate models to examine audit delay: one with LnLag as the dependent measure and the other with SLag. The results of the two models are similar. The coefficient of SIZEAUD1 is positive and statistically significant at p < 0.05 using LnLag as the dependent measure and p < 0.10 using SLag as the dependent measure. By comparison, SIZEAUD2 is insignificant (p > 0.10). The findings suggest that audit delay is positively associated with the review auditor’s narcissism, but not that of the engagement auditor. This result is not surprising because the review auditor is more senior than the engagement auditor, and, further, becomes more involved at a later stage of the audit. Thus, substantial issues raised by the review auditor have the potential to delay the audit’s completion. Our findings for the review auditor are consistent with H1.[Insert Table 7 about here.]The OLS results also indicate that the coefficient of SIZECFO is positive and statistically significant (p < 0.01), whereas the interaction terms are not significant. The positive coefficient of SIZECFO indicates that the CFO’s narcissism contributes to audit delay. In addition, we find that audit delay is negatively associated with having a Big 4 auditor (p < 0.01) and the percentage of shares held by large shareholders (p < 0.10), and positively associated with client size (p < 0.01) and clients’ growth in revenues (p < 0.05). 5.4. Discretionary AccrualsNext, we estimate the following OLS regression model to examine the effect of auditors’ narcissism on the outcome of auditor-client negotiations.DAit=α0+α1SIZEAUD1j+α2SIZEAUD2j+α3SIZECFOj+α4BIGit+α5SIZEit+α6GROWTHRit+α7LEVit+α8QUICKit+α9INVRECit+α10LOSSit+α11FOREIGNit+α12FIRMAGEit+α13BOARDINDit+ α14BOARDMEETit+α15TOPSHit+α16-21YearIndicators+IndustryIndicators+Error.The left-hand-side variable, DA, is the discretionary accruals estimated using the modified Jones model with firm profitability included for each year-industry group, conditional on having at least five firms in the group (Kothari et al. 2005). We run the model three times, using three different dependent measures: absolute discretionary accruals (|DA|), positive discretionary accruals (PosDA), and negative discretionary accruals (NegDA). The primary independent variables of interest, again, proxy for auditor narcissism, SIZEAUD1 and SIZEAUD2. As before, we include SIZECFO to control for the client’s narcissism and other control variables following prior literature (e.g., Ashbaugh-Skaife et al. 2008). All variables are defined previously in the model for audit delay.The right-hand side of Table 7 presents the results of three separate OLS regression models. Looking at the results using |DA| as the dependent measure, we find that SIZEAUD1 is negative and statistically significant at p < 0.01, whereas SIZEAUD2 is insignificant (p > 0.10). The absolute value of clients’ discretionary accruals is negatively associated with the review auditor’s narcissism, but not the engagement auditor’s narcissism. Again, the review auditor’s narcissism plays a significant role. We contend that smaller absolute discretionary accruals improve financial reporting quality and, in turn, decrease the auditor’s potential exposure. The findings for the review auditor are consistent with H2.We also document that the coefficient of SIZECFO is positive and statistically significant (p < 0.10), whereas the interaction terms are not significant. The positive coefficient of SIZECFO provides evidence that the CFO’s narcissism leads to larger absolute discretionary accruals. Further, absolute discretionary accruals are negatively associated with having a Big N auditor (p < 0.01) and clients’ size (p < 0.05), and positively associated with the percentage of shares held by large shareholders (p < 0.01) and clients’ age (p < 0.10).The results using PosDA as the dependent measure are similar to those discussed above, though significance levels are reduced. We find that SIZEAUD1 is negative and statistically significant at p < 0.05. The review auditor’s narcissism is negatively associated with clients’ positive discretionary accruals. So the review auditor’s narcissism leads to less aggressive financial reporting, consistent with H2. The results using NegDA as the dependent measure indicate that SIZEAUD1 is positive and statistically significant at p < 0.10. In this case, the review auditor’s narcissism is positively associated with clients’ negative discretionary accruals, which leads to more conservative financial reporting.Finally, we repeat the analyses using alternative measures of clients’ discretionary accruals. Specifically, we re-compute DA to allow for asymmetry in the timing of gain and loss recognition (see also Ball and Shivakumar 2006; Carver et al. 2011; Stanley et al. 2015). We also compute DA following Francis et al. (2005). The results (not tabulated), using the alternative measures, are consistent with those presented in Table 7. In every case, SIZEAUD1 is significant at p < 0.05, and the findings suggest that the review auditor’s narcissism is associated with less aggressive financial reporting. 6. Concluding RemarksThis paper reports the results of an experiment designed to examine the effect of auditor narcissism on auditor-client negotiations. The benefits of using an experimental approach are that we can observe the negotiation process and control for the influence of potentially confounding, extraneous factors. Our experimental findings indicate that auditor narcissism is associated with more impasses and longer negotiations. Notably, when agreements are reached, auditor narcissism leads to outcomes that are indicative of less aggressive reporting choices. To shed light on the generalizability of our findings to a naturally-occurring setting, we collect archival data and perform supplemental analyses. We provide evidence that auditor narcissism is positively associated with audit delay, taking longer to issue audited financial statements. We also find that auditor narcissism is associated with clients’ making less aggressive reporting choices. We document that auditor narcissism is linked to clients having smaller absolute and positive discretionary accruals and larger negative discretionary accruals. The supplemental results are consistent with our experimental findings, underscoring the role of auditor narcissism in negotiations.Our experimental findings suggest that narcissistic auditors-participants are more likely to hold their ground in negotiations, allowing them to reach agreements that reflect less aggressive reported amounts. This result suggests that auditor narcissism is potentially beneficial, as a means to promote audit quality, though it might undermine auditor-client relations. Accounting firms make employee selection decisions, including promotions, based on employees’ attributes (Campbell 2012). Such attributes oftentimes reflect personality dimensions (e.g., empathy, confidence, propensity for risk, etc.). Indeed, workplace personality testing is estimated to be a $500 million-a-year business, with annual growth rates of 10 to 15 percent (Weber and Dwoskin 2014). We suggest that auditor narcissism is an important attribute that merits attention: accounting firms may need to carefully consider its benefits and costs when making recruiting and promotion decisions in order to maximize overall audit effectiveness.Our study is subject to certain limitations, which provide potential avenues for future research. First, our negotiation setting is one in which the auditor and client have equal bargaining power. We acknowledge that situational factors may shift the balance of power toward one party (Gibbins et al. 2001; Brown and Wright 2008; Salterio 2012). Yet, we do not have any obvious reason to believe that our results for auditor narcissism would not apply to settings in which bargaining power is unequal. The issue, however, remains an empirical question. Second, our negotiation setting excludes interpersonal interactions, which are crucial in an audit context. To the extent that narcissistic auditors hold firm in negotiations, such behavior potentially may create ill will with the client. Future interactions may be tainted, and auditor-client relations may deteriorate over time. The auditor’s demeanor and tactics may not sit well with the client and, eventually, the auditor-client relationship may be severed. We encourage future study to explore this important issue.Relatedly, in our experimental setting auditor-client dyads interact in a series of one-shot negotiations, where the dyads are re-paired each period. Thus, negotiation outcomes in one period do not affect outcomes, including participants’ payoffs, in future periods. Yet, in a naturally-occurring setting, the auditor and client are involved in a multi-period relationship. Along these lines, McCracken et al. (2008) observe that, in auditor-client interactions, the audit partner assumes the role of relationship manager, responsible for keeping the client happy. The researchers collect field data and provide evidence that accounting firms manage the assignment of audit partners based on client (CFO) preferences. Poor relationships, irrespective of the underlying reason, provide a basis to remove partners from engagements. For our purposes, the client might apply pressure to have a narcissistic audit partner taken off the engagement. We leave this issue for future study. We use Chinese participants in our experiment. Chinese and Westerners could approach negotiations differently, primarily because the former emphasizes collectivism whereas the latter emphasizes individualism (e.g., Triandis 1986; Lee 2003). Lee et al. (2012) suggest that Chinese are generally concerned with relational harmony, mutual trust, and reciprocity in negotiations. However, quite apart from culture, we expect high narcissists to behave differently than low narcissists. That is, we expect the effects of narcissism on the negotiation process and outcomes to hold across cultures.Lastly, the applicability of our experimental findings to naturally-occurring settings rely on the assumption that our parameters capture critical features of the audit ecology. Though our setting is abstract, we exercise care in determining participants’ relative payoffs, contingent on their actions. Further, we take comfort that the archival findings mirror those obtained in the laboratory, providing evidence of external validity. Despite the limitations, our findings provide important insights into the effect of auditor narcissism on auditor-client interactions. Figure 1Players’ Payoffs per PeriodPanel A: Client Payoff28130505715Client’s payoff = Accepted value00Client’s payoff = Accepted value1707242158568Yes00Yes1416050126365508003175Does the period end with an agreement? (i.e., is there an accepted value?)00Does the period end with an agreement? (i.e., is there an accepted value?)14160501016000017907007620No00No287655085090Client’s payoff = Zero00Client’s payoff = ZeroPanel B: Auditor Payoff3566795182880Auditor’s payoff = 400 – 50% × (reported value – actual value)00Auditor’s payoff = 400 – 50% × (reported value – actual value)2960914169908Yes00Yes30988001435101841500202565Is the reported value higher than the actual value?00Is the reported value higher than the actual value?left4445Does the period end with an agreement? (i.e., is there an accepted reported value?)00Does the period end with an agreement? (i.e., is there an accepted reported value?)12991198890Yes00Yes311150080645135255036195309245066040No00No36830007620Auditor’s payoff = 40000Auditor’s payoff = 40013462001466851338036234587No00No1836965226060 Auditor’s payoff = Zero00 Auditor’s payoff = Zero Notes: The figure depicts participants’ payoffs in our experiment, contingent on negotiated outcomes.Table 1Relation between Potential and Actual Asset Value Value?Probability of being the actual value?Probability of being higher than the actual value10005%95%9505%90%9005%85%8505%80%8005%75%7505%70%7005%65%6505%60%6005%55%5505%50%5005%45%4505%40%4005%35%3505%30%3005%25%2505%20%2005%15%1505%10%1005%5%505%0%Notes: In our experiment, the client chooses a reported amount. The first column includes potential amounts that can be reported. The second column indicates that the probability that each potential amount corresponds with the actual asset value is 5 percent. The third column shows the probability that the reported amount is greater than the actual asset value.Table 2Descriptive StatisticsAuditor NarcissismClient NarcissismLowHighLowAGREE 0.86 (n=432)OFFERS 10.99 (n=370)VALUE 472.70 (n=370) AGREE 0.77 (n=486)OFFERS 14.34 (n=374)VALUE 529.95 (n=374)HighAGREE 0.74 (n=396)OFFERS 14.14 (n=294)VALUE 426.70 (n=294)AGREE 0.67 (n=450)OFFERS 14.38 (n=301)VALUE 464.45 (n=301) Notes: Auditor- and client-participants are categorized as high (H) or low (L) based on their responses to the narcissistic personality inventory. We establish auditor-client dyads such that narcissism pairings are HH, HL, LH, and LL, where the first letter represents the auditor and the second represents the client. The cell entries include averages for various measures, with the number of observations shown parenthetically. The measures are defined as follows: AGREE = the percentage of time that auditor-client dyads reach an agreement; OFFERS = the average number of offers before an agreement is reached; andVALUE = the average reported amount that is agreed upon.Table 3Experimental PredictionsHypothesisMeasureAuditor NarcissismPredicted SignH1: Auditor narcissism is positively associated with the length of the negotiation process.AGREEtH < L?OFFERStH > L+H2: Auditor narcissism is positively associated with negotiation outcomes that are consistent with auditors’ preferences.VALUEtH < L?Notes: The variables of interest are defined as follows: AGREE = the percentage of time that auditor-client dyads reach an agreement; OFFERS = the average number of offers before an agreement is reached; andVALUE = the average reported amount that is agreed upon.For auditor narcissism, H refers to auditor-participants who have High NPI scores and L refers to those who have Low NPI scores. The predicted sign represents the predicted difference between H and L auditors. Table 4. Experimental ResultsPanel A: Analyses of the Negotiation Process and OutcomeIndependent VariablesDependent VariableAGREEtOFFERStVALUEtAuditorN ?0.841*** (?4.60) 3.975*** (4.80)?28.837***(?3.17)ClientN ?0.479** (?2.71) 2.658*** (3.06) 42.978*** (4.75)AuditorN x ClientN 0.183 (0.78) ?2.794** (?2.26)?13.131(?1.00)1st?Offer ?0.002*** (?5.15) 0.013*** (6.51) 0.268*** (11.62)Constant 3.188*** (10.13) 1.534 (1.00)277.189*** (15.93)Observations 1,764 1,339 1,339Panel B: Simple Effects (DV=OFFERSt)Independent VariablesClients’ NarcissismLowHighAuditorN 3.634*** (4.41) 1.556** (1.70)1st?Offer 0.008*** (3.06) 0.017*** (5.86)Constant 5.410*** (2.88) 0.842 (0.38)Observations 664 675Notes: The cell entries include estimated coefficients (test statistics). For AGREE, we perform a logistic regression, controlling for period, so the values shown parenthetically are z-statistics. For the other dependent variables, we perform an ordinary least squares regression, so the values shown parenthetically are t-statistics. One, two, and three asterisks (*, **, and ***) indicate statistical significance at the 10 percent, 5 percent, and 1 percent levels, respectively. The p-values are one-tailed for AuditorN because we have directional predictions and two-tailed for the other variables. The independent variables are defined as follows. AuditorN and ClientN are auditors’ and clients’ narcissism, respectively, coded as 1 for High and 0 for Low; and 1st?Offer is the client’s first offer. For H1, we test whether auditor narcissism affects the negotiation process using two dependent variables, AGREEt and OFFERSt. AGREEt is defined as 1 if an agreement is reached and 0 otherwise; and OFFERSt represents the number of offers preceding an agreement. In Panel B, we partition the data based on clients’ narcissism (Low and High) and present the simple effects of the significant interaction term revealed in Panel A (for OFFERSt). For H2, we test whether auditor narcissism affects negotiated outcome using VALUEt as the dependent variable. VALUEt is defined as the agreed upon amount. Table 5. Variables in the Supplemental AnalysesVariable DefinitionLnLagThe natural log of the number of days between fiscal year-end and the audit report date;SLagThe number of days between fiscal year-end and the audit report date scaled by 360 days;DADiscretionary accruals estimated based on modified Jones model with firm profitability included (Kothari et al. 2005) for each year-industry group, conditional on having at least five firms in the group;SIZEAUD1Standardized signature size of the reviewing auditor;SIZEAUD2Standardized signature size of the engagement auditor;SIZECFOStandardized signature size of chief financial officer (CFO);BIG1 if a firm is audited by a Big 4 audit firm, 0 otherwise;SIZEThe natural log of the firm’s total assets;GROWTHR Revenues in year t minus revenues in year t-1 divided by revenues in year t-1;LEV Total debt divided by total assets;QUICK Current assets minus inventory divided by current liabilities;INVREC Inventory plus accounts receivable divided by total assets;LOSS 1 if a firm reports negative net earnings and 0 otherwise;FOREIGN 1 if a firm is involved in foreign operation and 0 otherwise;FIRMAGEThe natural log of the firm’s age.BOARDINDPercentage of independent directors at the end of year t;BOARDMEETNumber of board meetings in year t;TOPSHPercentage of shares held by the top ten shareholders at the end of year t;Notes: The first two variables, LNLAG and SLAG are dependent measures used to examine audit delay (H1). The third variable, DA, is used to compute dependent measures to examine clients’ discretionary accruals. For DA, we examine the absolute value of discretionary accruals (|DA|) as well as positive and negative discretionary accruals (PosDA and NegDA). The remainder are independent variables in the supplemental analyses.Table 6. Descriptive Statistics: Supplemental DataVariableMeanStd. Dev.25th Percentile50th Percentile75th PercentileLnLag4.4830.2574.3824.4664.682SLag|DA|0.0500.0470.0160.0370.067PosDA0.0510.0490.0150.0360.068NegDASIZEAUD10.2790.168.01780.2320.319SIZEAUD20.234.01150.1520.2070.289SIZECFO0.0850.0500.0510.0730.108SIZE22.3341.26021.49922.21023.162GROWTHR0.1480.499-0.0120.0820.206LEV0.5500.1900.4340.5630.684QUICK0.9850.8460.4920.7561.207INVREC0.2840.2070.1130.2410.426FIRMAGE2.6240.2992.3982.6392.890BOARDIND0.3650.0500.3330.3330.385BOARDMEET2.1800.3581.9462.1972.398TOPSH0.5240.1630.3980.5220.650Categorical VariablesBIG0.061LOSS0.089FOREIGN0.289Observations429Notes: The variables are defined in Table 5.Table 7. Results: Supplemental ResultsIndependentVariablesAudit DelayClients’ Discretionary AccrualsLnLagSLag|DA|PosDANegDASIZEAUD10.137**0.023*-0.044***-0.074**0.034*(2.03)(1.48)(-3.00)(-1.71)(1.49)SIZEAUD20.0480.0170.0050.0370.001(0.52)(0.77)(0.20)(0.99)(0.02)SIZECFO0.762***0.181***0.138*0.153-0.171(3.15)(3.15)(1.83)(1.32)(-1.51)SIZEAUD1*SIZECFO-1.807-0.3850.3430.273-0.31(-0.88)(-0.82)(0.78)(0.24)(-0.42)SIZEAUD2*SIZECFO-2.574-0.61-0.0662.628*1.577 (-0.83)(-0.83)(-0.09)(1.79)(1.54)BIG-0.115**-0.034***-0.022***-0.031*0.008(-2.21)(-3.28)(-2.74)(-1.85)(0.57)SIZE0.044***0.009***-0.005**-0.0070.010**(3.68)(3.51)(-2.10)(-1.24)(2.27)GROWTH0.047**0.011**0.0070.0110.004(2.30)(2.29)(1.13)(1.33)(0.69)LEV-0.035-0.0020.005-0.035-0.033(-0.28)(-0.08)(0.24)(-0.94)(-1.00)QUICK0.0070.0010.0040.0090.002(0.29)(0.25)(0.68)(0.92)(0.28)INVREC0.009-0.0020.0070.005-0.005(0.10)(-0.10)(0.38)(0.19)(-0.18)LOSS0.0550.0150.0140.011-0.001(1.29)(1.62)(1.60)(0.60)(-0.10)FOREIGN0.0430.0090.0010.007-0.002(1.50)(1.50)(0.24)(0.55)(-0.28)FIRMAGE-0.0060.0000.020*0.022-0.024(-0.13)(0.01)(1.83)(1.08)(-1.60)BOARDIND-0.304-0.0730.0460.088-0.150**(-1.19)(-1.39)(0.96)(1.00)(-2.36)BOARDMEET-0.059-0.0120.0020.000-0.01(-1.58)(-1.56)(0.26)(-0.01)(-0.98)TOPSH-0.209**-0.039*0.054***0.066**-0.043(-2.14)(-1.96)(2.74)(2.02)(-1.47)Constant3.736***0.0780.0840.015-0.153*(13.72)(1.29)(1.33)(0.12)(-1.69)Adjusted R-squared0.2320.1820.1030.1720.084Observations429429429189240Notes: The left-hand side of the table (columns 1 and 2) reports the OLS regression results using audit delay (LnLag and SLag) as the dependent variable. The right-hand side of the table (columns 3 – 5) reports the OLS regression results using discretionary accruals (|DA|, PosDA, and NegDA) as the dependent variable. The variables are defined in Table 5. Though not shown, we also include year and industry fixed effects. In addition, standard errors are adjusted for clustering at the firm level. The table entries represent the estimated coefficient (t-statistic). One, two, and three asterisks *, **, and ***) indicate statistical significance at the 10 percent, 5 percent, and 1 percent levels, respectively. The p-value are two-tailed, excepting SIZEAUD1 and SIZEAUD2. The p-values are one-tailed for the auditor narcissism variables as we have directional predictions. ReferencesAkers, M.D., D.E. Giacomino, and J. Weber. 2014. Narcissism in public accounting firms. Accounting and Finance Research 3(3): 170–178.American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. Antle, R., and B. Nalebuff. 1991. Conservatism and auditor-client negotiations. Journal of Accounting Research 29(Supplement): 31–54.Ashbaugh-Skaife, H., D.W. Collins, W.R. Kinney, and R. LaFond. 2008. The effect of SOX internal control deficiencies on their remediation on audit quality. The Accounting Review 83(1): 217–250.Ashton, R.H., J.J. Willingham, and R.K. Elliott. 1987. An empirical analysis of audit delay. Journal of Accounting Research 25(2): 275-–292.Ashton, R.H., P.R. Graul, and J.D. Newton. 1989. Audit delay the timeliness of corporate reporting. Contemporary Accounting Research 5(2): 657—673.Babcock, L.H., and G. Loewenstein. 2005. Explaining bargaining impasse: The role of self-serving bias. In M.H. Bazerman (Ed.) Negotiation, Decision Making and Conflict Management, Vol. 1 – 3 (231–248). Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Publishing. Ball, R., and L. Shivakumar. 2006. The role of accruals in asymmetrically timely gain and loss recognition. Journal of Accounting Research 44(2): 207–242.Bame-Aldred, C.W., and T. Kida. 2007. A comparison of auditor and client initial positions and tactics. Accounting, Organizations and Society 32: 497–511.Barry, B., and R.A. Friedman. 1998. Bargainer characteristics in distributive and integrative negotiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74(2): 345–359.Bauer, T.D. 2015. The effects of client identity strength and professional identity salience on auditor judgments. The Accounting Review 90(1): 95–114.Baumeister, R.F., B.J. Bushman, and W.K. Campbell. 2000. Self-esteem, narcissism, and aggression: Does violence result from low self-esteem or from threatened egotism? Current Directions in Psychological Science 9(1): 26–29. Beattie, V., S. Fearnley, and R. Brandt. 2000. Behind the audit report: A descriptive study of discussions and negotiations between auditors and directors. International Journal of Auditing 4(2): 177–202.Bhaskar, L., T. Majors, and A.M. Vitalis. 2015. Are “good” auditors impacted more by ego depletion? Threats to valued auditor attributes. Working paper, Indiana University (December).Birnberg, J.G., M.D. Shields, and S.M. Young. 1990. The case for multiple methods in empirical management accounting research (with an illustration from budget setting). Journal of Management Accounting Research 2: 33–66.Bolton, G. 1991. A comparative model of bargaining: Theory and evidence. The American Economic Review 81(5): 1096–1136.Boone, J.P., I.K. Khurana, and K.K. Raman. 2011. Litigation risk and abnormal accruals. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 30(2): 231–256.Brewer, J., and A. Hunter. 2006. Foundations of Multimethod Research: Synthesizing Styles. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Brislin, R.W. 1970. Back-Translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 1(3): 185–216.Brown, H.L., and A.M. Wright. 2008. Negotiation research in auditing. Accounting Horizons 22(1): 91–109. Brown, H.L., and K.M. Johnstone. 2009. Resolving disputed financial reporting issues: Effects of auditor negotiation experience and engagement risk on negotiation process and outcome. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 28(2): 65–92.Brown-Liburd, H.L., A.M. Wright, and T. Zamora. 2016. Managers’ strategic reporting judgments in audit negotiations. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 35(1): 47–64.Calegari, M.J., J.W. Schatzberg, and G.R. Sevcik. 1998. Experimental evidence of differential auditor pricing and reporting strategies. The Accounting Review 73(2): 255–275.Campbell, D. 2012. Employee selection as a control system. Journal of Accounting Research 50(4): 931–966.Campbell, W.K., C.P. Bush, A.B. Brunell, and J. Shelton. 2005. Understanding the social cost of narcissism: The case of the tragedy of the commons. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31(10): 1358–1368. Carnevale, P.J., and D.G. Pruitt. 1992. Negotiation and mediation. Annual Review of Psychology 43: 531–582.Carroll, L. 1987. A study of narcissism, affiliation, intimacy, and power motives among students in business administration. Psychological Reports 61(2): 355–358.Carver, B.T., C.W. Hollingsworth, and J.D. Stanley. 2011. Recent auditor downgrade activity and changes in clients’ discretionary accruals. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 30(3): 33–58. Chatterjee, A., D.C. Hambrick. 2007. It’s all about me: Narcissistic chief executive officers and their effects on company strategy and performance. Administrative Science Quarterly 52(3): 351–386. Chen, S., S. Y. Sun, and D. Wu. 2010. Client importance, institutional improvements, and audit quality in China: An office and individual auditor level analysis. The Accounting Review 85(1): 127–158.Chertkoff, J.M., and M. Conley. 1967. Opening offer and frequency of concessions as bargaining strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 7(2): 181–185.Chinese Ministry of Finance. 2001. Notice of issues regarding CPAs’ signatures and stamps on audit reports. Chinese Ministry of Finance, File No. [2001] 1035, July 2nd.Church, B.K., M. Peytcheva, W. Yu, and O.-A. Singtokul. 2015. Perspective taking in auditor-manager interactions: An experimental investigation of auditor behavior. Accounting, Organizations and Society 45: 40–51.Davis, M.H. 1980. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology 10: 85–104.De Dreu, C.K.W., L.R. Weingart, and S. Kwon. 2000. Influence of social motives on integrative negotiation: A meta-analytic review and test of two theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78(5): 889–905.Dhawan, N., M.E. Kunik, J. Oldham, and J. Coverdale. 2010. Prevalence and treatment of narcissistic personality disorder in the community: A systematic review. Comprehensive Psychiatry 51(4): 333–339.Dickinson, K.A., and A.L. Pincus. 2003. Interpersonal analysis of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. Journal of Personality Disorders 17(3): 188–207.Einhorn, H., and R. Hogarth. 1981. Behavioral decision theory: Processes of judgment and choice. Annual Review of Psychology 32: 53–88.Emmons, R.A. 1984. Factor analysis and construct validity of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment 48(3): 291–300.Emmons, R.A. 1987. Narcissism: Theory and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52(1): 11–17.Ettredge, M.L., C. Li, and L. Sun. 2006. The impact of SOX Section 404 internal control quality assessment on audit delay in the SOX era. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 25(2): 1–23.Fischbacher, U. 2007. Z-tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics 10(2): 171–178.Fitzgerald, B.C., C.J. Wolfe, and K.W. Smith. 2015. Management’s preference: Can auditors stop it from biasing accounting estimates? Working paper, Northeastern University and Texas A&M University. Foster, J.D., and W.K. Campbell. 2007. Are there such things as ‘‘narcissists’’ in social psychology? A taxometric analysis of the narcissistic personality inventory. Personality and Individual Differences 439(6): 1321–1332.Foster, J.D., W.K. Campbell, and J.M. Twenge. 2003. Individual differences in narcissism: Inflated self-views across the lifespan and around the world. Journal of Research in Personality 37(6): 469–486.Firth, M., P. H. Malatesta, Q. Xin, and L. Xu. 2012. Corporate investment, government control, and financing channels: Evidence from China’s listed companies. Journal of Corporate Finance 18(3): 433–450.Francis, J., R. LaFond, P. Olsson, and K. Schipper. 2005. The market pricing of accruals quality. Journal of Accounting and Economics 39(2): 295–327. Furnham, A., and J. Crump. 2014. A big-five facet analysis of sub-clinical narcissism: Understanding boldness in terms of well-known personality traits. Personality and Mental Health 8(3): 209–217.Galinsky, A.D., and T. Musweiler. 2001. First offers as anchors: The role of perspective-taking and negotiator focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81(4): 657–669.Gibbins, M., S.A. McCracken, and S.E. Salterio. 2005. Negotiations over accounting issues: The congruency of audit partner and chief financial officer. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 24(Supplement): 171–193.Gibbins, M., S.A. McCracken, and S.E. Salterio. 2007. The chief financial officer’s perspective on auditor-client negotiations. Contemporary Accounting Research 24(2): 387–422.Gibbins, M., S.A. McCracken, and S.E. Salterio. 2010. The auditor’s strategy selection for negotiation with management: flexibility of initial accounting position and nature of the relationship. Accounting, Organizations and Society 35(6): 579–595.Gibbins, M., S. Salterio, and A. Webb. 2001. Evidence about auditor-client management negotiation concerning client’s financial reporting. Journal of Accounting Research 31(3): 535–563.Golec de Zavala, A., A. Cichocka, and I. Iskra-Golec. 2013. Collective narcissism moderates the effect of in-group image threat on intergroup hostility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 104(6): 1019–1039.Greenhalgh, L. 1987. Relationships in negotiation. Negotiation Journal 3(3): 235–243. Greenhalgh, L., and R.W. Gilkey. 1997. Clinical assessment methods in negotiation research: The study of narcissism and negotiator effectiveness. Groups Decision and Negotiation 6(4): 289–316.Grijalva, E., and P.D. Harms. 2014. Narcissism: An integrative synthesis and dominance complementarity model. Academy of Management Perspectives 28(2): 108–127.Gul, F.A., D. Wu, and Z. Yang. 2013. Do individual auditors affect audit quality? Evidence from archival data. The Accounting Review 88(6): 1993–2023. Hageman, A.M. 2008. A review of the strengths and weaknesses of archival, behavioral, and qualitative research methods: recognizing the potential benefits of triangulation. Advances in Accounting Behavioral Research 11: 1–30. Hales, J., J.L. Hobson, and R. Resuteck. 2012. The dark side of socially mediated rewards: How narcissism and social status affect managerial reporting. Working paper, Georgia Tech (March).Ham, C., N. Seybert, and S. Wang. 2014. Narcissism is a bad sign: CEO signature size, investment, and performance. Working paper, University of Maryland (June).Ham, C., M. Lang, N. Seybert, and S. Wang. 2015. CFO narcissism and financial reporting quality. Working paper, University of Maryland (April).Heifetz, A., and E. Segev. 2004. The evolutionary role of toughness in bargaining. Games and Economic Behavior 49(1): 117–134.Hepper, E.G., C.M. Hart, R. Meek, S. Sicek, and C. Sedikides. 2014. Narcissism and empathy in young offenders and non-offenders. European Journal of Personality 28(2): 201–210. Hollindale, J., P. Kent, and R. McNamara. 2011. Auditor tactics in negotiations: A research note. International Journal of Auditing 15: 288–300.Holt, C.A, and S.K. Laury. 2002. Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review 92(5): 1644–1655.Hopwood, C.W., M.B. Donnellan, R.A. Ackerman, K.M. Thomas, L.C. Morey, and A.E. Skodol. 2013. The validity of the personality diagnostic questionnaire-4 narcissistic personality disorder scale for assessing pathological grandiosity. Journal of Personality Assessment 95(3): 274–283.Hüffmeier, J., P.E. Freund, A. Zerres, K. Backhaus, and G. Hertel. 2014. Being tough or being nice? A meta-analysis on the impact of hard- and softline strategies in distributive negotiations. Journal of Management 40(3): 866–892.Hui, C.H., and H.C. Triandis. 1985. Measurement in cross-cultural psychology. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 16(2): 131–152.Hurtt, R.K. 2010. Development of a scale to measure professional skepticism. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 29(1): 149–171.Hyder, E.B., M.J. Prietula, and L.R. Weingart. 2000. Getting to best: Efficiency versus optimality in negotiation. Cognitive Science 24(2): 169–204. Jick, T.D. 1979. Mixing qualitative and quantitative methods: Triangulation in action. Administrative Science Quarterly 24: 602–611.Johnson, E.N., J.R. Kuhn, Jr., B.A. Apostolou, and J.M. Hassell. 2013. Auditor perceptions of client narcissism as a fraud attitude risk factor. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 32(1): 203–219.Jones, D.N., and A.L. Neria. 2015. The dark triad and dispositional aggression. Personality and Individual Differences 86: 60–64. Jorgenson, D.O. 1977. Signature size and dominance: A brief note. Journal of Psychology 97(2): 269–270.Judd, J.S., K.J. Olsen, and J.M. Stekelberg. 2015. CEO narcissism, accounting quality, and external audit fees. Working paper, University of Arizona (May).Kong, D.T. 2015. Narcissists’ negative perceptions of their counterpart’s competence and benevolence and their own reduced trust in a negotiation context. Personality and Individual Differences 74(February): 196–201.Kothari, S. P., A. J. Leone, and C. E. Wasley. 2005. Performance matched discretionary accrual measures. Journal of Accounting and Economics 39(1): 163–197.Kubarych. T.S, I.J. Deary, and E.J. Austin. 2004. The narcissistic personality inventory: Factor structure in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences 36(4): 857–872.Küfner, A.C.P., S. Nestler, and M.D. Back. 2013. The two pathways to being an (un-)popular narcissist. Journal of Personality 81(2): 184–195. Kwan, V.S.Y., L.L. Kuang, and N.H.H. Hui. 2009. Identifying the sources of self-esteem: The mixed medley of benevolence, merit, and bias. Self and Identity 8(2-3): 176–195.Lee, C. 2003. Cowboys and Dragons: Shattering Cultural Myths to Advance Chinese-American Business. Chicago, IL: Dearborn Trade Publishing.Lee, S., J. Brett, and J. H. Park. 2012. East Asians’ social heterogeneity: Differences in norms among Chinese, Japanese, and Koreans negotiators. Negotiation Journal 28(4): 429–452.Lei, Y., J. Wu, and H. Sun. 2013. Impact of internal control audit on earnings quality: Evidence from listed companies in Shanghai Stock Exchange. Accounting Research 11: 75–81 (in Chinese).Lennox, C.S., X. Wu, and T. Zhang. 2014. Does mandatory rotation of audit partners improve audit quality? The Accounting Review 89(5): 1775–1803.Liu, X., and W. Chi. 2014. Does disclosure of CPAs’ names improve audit quality? Evidence from clients’ accounting restatements. The Chinese Certified Public Accountant 3: 56-64 (in Chinese).Luchner, A.F., J.M. Houston, C. Walker, and M.A. Houston. 2011. Exploring the relationship between two forms of narcissism and competitiveness. Personality and Individual Differences 51(6): 779–782.Ma, Z., and A. Jaeger. 2005. Getting to yes in China: Exploring personality effects in Chinese negotiation style. Group Decision and Negotiation 14(5): 415–437.Majors, T. 2015. The interaction of communicating uncertainty and the dark triad on managers’ reporting decisions. Working paper, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign (April).Mayhew, B.W., J.W. Schatzberg, and G.R. Sevcik. 2004. Examining the role of auditor quality and retained ownership in IPO markets: Experimental Evidence. Contemporary Accounting Research 21(1): 89–130.Mazei, J., J. Hüffmeier, P.A. Freund, A.F. Stuhlmacher, L. Bilke, and G. Hertel. 2015. A meta-analysis on gender differences in negotiation outcomes and their moderators. Psychological Bulletin 141(1): 85–104.McCracken, S.A., S.E. Salterio, and M. Gibbins. 2008. Auditor-client management relationships and roles in negotiating financial reporting. Accounting, Organizations and Society 33(4/5): 362–383.McCracken, S.A., S.E. Salterio, and M. Gibbins. 2011. Do managers intend to use the same negotiation strategies as partners? Behavioral Research in Accounting 23(1): 131–160.Miller, J.D., and W.K. Campbell. 2008. Comparing clinical and social-personality conceptualizations of narcissism. Journal of Personality 76(3): 449–476.Miller, J.D., J. McCain, D.R. Lynam, L.R. Few, B. Gentile, J. MacKillop, and W.K. Campbell. 2014. A comparison of the criterion validity of popular measures of narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder via the use of expert ratings. Psychological Assessments 26(3): 958–969.Morf, C.C., and F. Rhodewalt. 2001. Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychological Inquiry 12(4): 176–191. Nevicka, B., A.H.B. De Hoogh, A.E.M. Van Vianen, B. Beersma, and D. McIlwain. 2011. All I need is a stage to shine: Narcissists’ leader emergence and performance. Leadership Quarterly 22(5): 910–925.O’Neill, T.A., and N.J. Allen. 2014. Team task conflict resolution: An examination of its linkage to team personality composition and team effectiveness outcome. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice 18(2): 159–173.O’Reilly, C.A., B. Doer, D.F. Caldwell, and J.A. Chatman. 2014. Narcissistic CEOs and executive compensation. Leadership Quarterly 25(2): 218–234. Park, S.W., J. Ferrero, C.R. Colvin, and D.R. Carney. 2013. Narcissism and negotiation: Economic gain and interpersonal loss. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 35(6): 569–574.Quadackers, L., T. Groot, and A. Wright. 2014. Auditors’ professional skepticism: Neutrality versus presumptive doubt. Contemporary Accounting Research 31(3): 639–657.Raskin, R., and J. Novacek. 1989. An MMPI description of the narcissistic personality. Journal of Personality Assessment 53(1): 66–80.Raskin, R., and H. Terry. 1988. A principal-components analysis of the narcissistic personality inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54(5): 890–902.Resick, C.J., D.S. Whitman, S.M. Weingarden, and N.J. Hiller. 2009. The bright-side and the dark-side of CEO personality: Examining core self-evaluations, narcissism, transformational leadership, and strategic influence. Journal of Applied Psychology 94(6): 1365–1381.Rhodewalt, F., and C.C. Morf. 1995. Self and interpersonal correlates with of the narcissistic personality inventory: A review and new findings. Journal of Personality Research 29(1): 1–23.Ronningstam, E. 2009. Narcissistic personality disorder: Facing DSM-V. Psychiatric Annals 39(3): 111–121.Ryckman, R.M., B. Thornton, and J.C. Butler. 1994. Personality correlates of the hypercompetitive attitude scale: Validity tests of Horney’s theory of neurosis. Journal of Personality Assessment 62(1): 84–94.Salterio, S.E. 2012. Fifteen years in the trenches. Auditor-client negotiations exposed and explored. Accounting and Finance 52(Supplement): 233–286.Sandy, S.V., S.K. Boardman, and M. Deutsch. 2006. Personality and conflict. In M. Deutsch, P.T. Coleman, and E.C. Marcus (Eds.) The Handbook of Conflict Resolution, Second Edition (331–355). Hoboken, NJ: Jossey-Bass.Scandura, T.A., and E.A. Williams. 2000. Research methodology in management: Current practices, trends, and implications for future research. Academy of Management Journal 43(6): 1248–1264.Scofield, S.B., T.J. Phillips, Tand C.D. Bailey. 2004. An empirical reanalysis of the selection-socialization hypothesis: A research note. Accounting, Organizations and Society 29(5/6): 543–563.Smith, H.W. 1975. Strategies of social research: The methodological imagination. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Stanley, J.D., D.M. Brandon, and J.J. McMillan. 2015. Does lowballing impair audit quality? Evidence from client accruals surrounding analyst forecasts. Journal of Accounting and Public Policy 34(6): 625–645.Triandis, H. 1986. Collectivism vs. individualism: A reconceptualization of a basic concept in cross-cultural psychology. In C. Bagley and G. Verma (Eds.) Personality, Cognition, and Values: Cross-Cultural Perspectives of Childhood and Adolescence (223–249). London: Macmillan Publishers.Trotman, K.T., A.M. Wright, and S. Wright. 2009. An examination of the effects of auditor rank on pre-negotiation judgments. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 28(1): 191–203.Twenge, J. M., S. Konrath, J.D. Foster, W.K. Campbell, and B.J. Bushman. 2008. Egos inflating over time: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality 76(4): 875–901.Wang, K.J. 2010. Negotiating a fair value under accounting uncertainty: A laboratory experiment. Behavioral Research in Accounting 22(1): 109–134.Wang, K.J., and B.M. Tuttle. 2009. The impact of auditor rotation on auditor-client negotiation. Accounting, Organizations and Society 34(2): 222–243. Wang, Y., L. Yu, and Y. Zhao. 2015. The association between audit-partner quality and engagement quality: Evidence from financial report misstatements. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 34(3): 81–112. Watson, P.J., S.O. Grisham, M.V. Trotter, and M.D. Biderman. 1984. Narcissism and empathy: Validity evidence for the narcissistic personality inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment 48(3): 301–305.Watson, P.J., T. Little, S. Sawrie, and M.D. Biderman. 1992. Measures of the narcissistic personality: Complexity of relationships with self-esteem and empathy. Journal of Personality Disorder 6(4): 434–449.Watson, P.J., R.J. Morris, and L. Miller. 1997–1998. Narcissism and the self as continuum: Correlations with assertiveness and hypercompetitiveness. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality 17(3): 249–259.Weber, L., and E. Dwoskin. 2014. Are workplace personality tests fair? Wall Street Journal, September 29, available for download at . Weingart, L.R., E.B. Hyder, and M.J. Prietula. 1996. Knowledge matters: the effect of tactical descriptions on negotiation behavior and outcome. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70(6): 1205–1217.Whitworth, J.D., and T.A. Lambert. 2104. Office-level characteristics of the Big 4 and audit report timeliness. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 33(3): 129–152.Wink, P. 1991. Two faces of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61(4): 590–597. Zweigenhaft, R. L. 1970. Signature size: A key to status awareness. Journal of Social Psychology 81(1): 49–54.Zweigenhaft, R. L. 1977. The empirical study of signature size. Social Behavior and Personality 5(1): 177–185.Zweigenhaft, R.L., and D. Marlowe. 1973. Signature size: Studies in expressive movement.?Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 40(3): 469–473. ................
................
In order to avoid copyright disputes, this page is only a partial summary.
To fulfill the demand for quickly locating and searching documents.
It is intelligent file search solution for home and business.
Related searches
- pay unc tuition
- unc minimally invasive gynecologic surgery
- unc employee salary lookup
- charlotte observer charlotte water salaries
- charlotte plastic surgery charlotte nc
- unc minimally invasive gyn
- unc head and neck oncology
- unc endocrine surgery
- unc thread chart pdf
- unc job application status
- 8 32 unc 2a thread specifications
- unc thread size chart